1Department of Nursing Faculty of Health, Hiroshima Cosmopolitan University, Japan
2Institute of Biomedical & Health Sciences, Hiroshima University, Japan
Received date: November 16, 2015; Accepted date: December 18, 2015;5 Published date: December 25, 2015
Citation: Muraki S, Kaneko F, Okamura H (2015) Study on Factors Associated with Repeated Gambling Among Gamblers Anonymous Participants . J Addict Res Ther 6:251. doi:0.4172/2155-6105.1000251
Copyright: © 2015 Okamura H et al., This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Visit for more related articles at Journal of Addiction Research & Therapy
Objective: While there are some Gamblers Anonymous (GA) participants who gamble again, no studies have investigated factors that differentiate GA participants who can quit repeated gambling from those who gamble again. The purpose of this study was to investigate whether individuals who participated in GA, a self-help groups, gambled again, and to identify the factors that were associated with those who gambled again after participating in GA.
Methods: The subjects were 58 pathological gamblers who had participated in GA group meetings. We divided the subjects into 2 groups, a group who had gambled again and group who had not, and we investigated associations with their basic information, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory Trait Version (STAI-T), Generalized Self Efficacy Scale (GSES), and Effortful Control Scale for Adults (ECSA).
Results: The replies regarding whether they had gambled again after participating in GA showed that 25 (43.1%) had not and 33 had (56.9%). A logistic regression analysis showed that GSES and ECSA “inhibitory control” as significant factors that were associated with whether a subject returned to gambling.
Conclusion: These results suggested that it may be possible to identify people who have a temperament that makes them tend to gamble again by evaluating their self-efficacy and inhibitory control.
Gamblers anonymous; Self-help group; Gambling; Anxiety; Self-efficacy; Effortful control; Inhibitory control
People who participate in public gambling in Japan (hereinafter, gamblers) are estimated to include 16.7 million pachinko gamblers, 14.9 million horse race gamblers, 2.5 million boat race gamblers, and 2 million bicycle race gamblers [1]. Some gamblers can never resist the desire and drive for gambling and gamble repeatedly without taking any breaks, resulting in having social life problems such as multiple debts, unemployment, and family collapse. This state is called gambling disorder in the DSM-5 [2]. The number of gambling disorder estimated from the adult population in Japan is as many as 5.36 million (4.8% of adult population) in total, including 4.38 million men (8.7% of adult male) and 0.98 million women (1.8% of adult female) (Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare). Treatment takes a few years and its purpose is to make patients quit gambling. However, many of the patients succumb to their addiction, and it can be said that this gambling disorder is a disease that is difficult to completely cure.
An effective therapeutic approach for gambling disorder is psychological therapies [3]. However, at present, there are a small number of specialized medical institutions that provide psychological therapies for this disease. Gamblers Anonymous (GA) actually plays a psychotherapy-like role [4]. GA is a self-help group aiming at recovery from gambling addiction, in which anonymity, i.e., no need to reveal the real names of participants, is ensured and regular meetings (4 to 12 times per month) are held [5]. In the meetings, an environment is created in which participants “just speak, just listen, and do not discuss” so that they can talk without anxiety. Furthermore, the participants implement “12 steps for recovery” such as “having an insight into the disease, reviewing failure, and making amends for wrongs” to prevent gambling addictive behavior, to recover their humanity, and to improve their way of life [6]. Studies of GA have reported the effect of participation in GA on the inhibition of gambling behavior [7,8] or the effect after the introduction of cognitive-behavioral therapy [9,10]. However, while there are some GA participants who gamble again, no studies have investigated factors that differentiate GA participants who can quit repeated gambling from those who gamble again.
Therefore, this study examined the presence or absence of repeated gambling, the degree of gambling addiction, the state of anxiety, attentional control ability, and self-efficacy in gambling addicts participating in GA to investigate what factors, particularly what mental state factors, were associated with those who gambled again after participating in GA.
There are 158 GA groups in Japan, and the total number of GA members is not clear. In this study, eligibility criteria for subjects were GA members participating in 2 GA groups in Hiroshima prefecture in Japan who regularly participated in GA meetings at least once a month. In addition, exclusion criteria were those who were hospitalized with or attending hospital for psychiatric disorders and who were not allowed to participate by their physicians at the time of the survey.
1) Subjects’ characteristics
A self-report questionnaire was used to assess the age, sex, the presence or absence of repeated gambling, and if present, the number of repeated gambling episodes after participating in GA, the type of gambling, the duration of gambling, the duration of participation in GA, and the presence or absence of debt with the total debt from all source.
2) State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)
The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) is a self-assessment questionnaire created by Spielberger et al. [11] to measure the degree of anxiety, consisting of 20 items assessing state anxiety and another 20 assessing trait anxiety. This study used the trait anxiety scale to measure whether natural personality trait anxiety affects addictive behavior. Each item is answered on a 4-point scale ranging from “almost never (1 point)” to “always (4 points).” The total score is calculated and higher total scores indicate higher trait anxiety.
3) Effortful Control Scale for Adults (ECSA)
The Effortful Control Scale for Adults (ECSA) is a conceptual scale of individual differences in attentional control ability, which is the Japanese translated version [12] of the 35-item Effortful Control (EC) Scale of the Adult Temperament Questionnaire created by Rothbart et al. [13]. The ECSA is a self-assessment questionnaire consisting of a total of 35 question items including 1) 11 items of inhibitory control (capacity to suppress inappropriate approach behavior), 2) 12 items of activation control (capacity to perform an action when there is a strong tendency to avoid it), and 3) 12 items of attentional control (capacity to focus attention as well as to shift attention when desired). Each item is answered on a 4-point scale ranging from “no (1 point)” to “yes (4 points).” The total score is calculated for each subscale, and higher total scores indicate higher control ability. The previous study examined the relationship between the EC Scale and problem behavior before adulthood and revealed that it is not incompatible with psychiatric disorders [12].
4) General Self-Efficacy Scale (GSES)
The General Self-Efficacy Scale (GSES) is a self-assessment questionnaire created by Sakano et al. [14], consisting of 16 items to measure the level of individual recognition of general self-efficacy. The GSES can assess whether one can perform appropriate coping behavior and problem-solving behavior without impairment of physical and mental health under stressful conditions. Each item is answered on a two-point scale: “Yes (1 point)” or “No (0 point).” Higher scores indicate higher self-efficacy.
The survey was conducted by distributing and collecting the questionnaires. Researchers participated in open GA meetings in which non-members could participate and distributed documents including those describing an outline of the study, consent form, and self-administered questionnaires. Participants who consented were asked to complete the questionnaires on the same day during the meetings, and the completed questionnaires were collected at once. In addition, if there were members absent from the GA meeting, the group leader was asked to distribute a letter explaining the purpose of the study and the self-administered questionnaires. These members were asked to complete the questionnaires at home and send the completed questionnaires to the researcher.
To examine the relationship of the subjects’ characteristics and important factors for preventing repeated gambling to the state of gambling addiction, trait anxiety, attentional control ability, and self-efficacy, the subjects were divided into two groups: those who gambled again after participating in GA and those who did not. The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used to test normality, and then the relationship with trait anxiety (STAI), attentional control ability (ECSA), and self-efficacy (GSES) was tested using the t-test, Mann-Whitney U-test, and logistic regression analysis.
For the analysis, IBM SPSS ver.22.0 for Windows was used. All tests were two-sided, and the level of significance was set at P < 0.05.
This study was conducted with the approval of the Ethical Review Board of the Graduate School of Health Sciences, Hiroshima University. Before the study, we participated in open GA meetings and explained in writing to the subjects the purpose, methods, and details of the study, possibility of withdrawal from the study at any time, absence of any disadvantages accruing from withdrawal, strict protection of privacy, and method of publication of the results. The survey was conducted only among GA participants who consented.
Participation of the subjects in the study
The questionnaires and a consent form were distributed to 72 GA members who met the eligibility criteria and consented in 7 GA groups, and 61 (84.7%) responded to the questionnaires. Those with missing data were excluded, and the final analysis subjects consisted of 58 members (80.6%). We could not get information about the 14 members who were excluded.
Overview of the subjects
Table 1 shows the age, gender, type of gambling, duration of gambling, duration of participation in GA (number of months of participation in GA), presence or absence, and number of times, of repeated gambling after participating in GA, presence or absence of debt, and the total debt from all sources. Pachinko was the most common types of gambling (n = 51, 87.9%). Twenty-five subjects (43.1%) did not gamble again after participating in GA, while 33 (56.9%) gambled again. One or two was the most common number of times of repeated gambling (n = 17, 29.3%), followed by 10 or more (n = 9, 15.5%).
N | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Age (years) | 20 - 29 | 7 | 12.10% | |
30 - 39 | 11 | 19.00% | ||
40 - 49 | 17 | 29.30% | ||
50 - 59 | 16 | 12.60% | ||
>= 60 | 7 | 12.10% | ||
Mean (SD) | 45.4 (11.7) | |||
Gender | Male | 54 | 93.10% | |
Female | 4 | 6.90% | ||
Type of gambling | Pachinko | 51 | 87.90% | |
Horse race | 5 | 8.60% | ||
Boat race | 1 | 1.70% | ||
Foreign exchange | 1 | 1.70% | ||
Duration of gambling (years) | 1-9 | 9 | 15.50% | |
10-19 | 10 | 17.20% | ||
20 - 29 | 21 | 36.20% | ||
>= 30 | 18 | 31.00% | ||
Mean (SD) | 22.6 (10.4) | |||
Repeated gambling after participating in GA (times) | none | 25 | 43.10% | |
1-2 | 17 | 29.30% | ||
3-5 | 5 | 8.60% | ||
6-9 | 2 | 3.40% | ||
>= 10 | 9 | 15.50% | ||
Debt (ten thousand yen) | none | 4 | 6.90% | |
1-99 | 4 | 6.90% | ||
100 - 99 | 24 | 41.40% | ||
500 - 999 | 15 | 25.90% | ||
>= 1000 | 11 | 19.00% | ||
Mean (SD) | 713.7 (1221.5) | |||
Number of months of participation in GA | < 12 | 16 | 27.60% | |
24-24 | 11 | 19.00% | ||
24 - 60 | 17 | 29.30% | ||
>= 60 | 14 | 24.10% | ||
Mean (SD) | 39.4 (36.7) |
Table 1: Demographic profile
Factors associated with the presence or absence of repeated gambling -univariate analysis
The subjects’ characteristics and assessment items were compared between the 31 subjects who gambled again and the 27 who did not. As a result, there were significant differences between the two groups in the following 8 items (Table 2): STAI (P = 0.004), GSES (P < 0.001), ECSA (inhibitory control) (P = 0.001), ECSA (activation control) (P = 0.004), duration of gambling (number of years) (P = 0.001), age (P = 0.001), GA 20 questions (P = 0.046), and ECSA (attentional control) (P = 0.024).
Repeated gambling | N | Mean (SD) | Pa | |
---|---|---|---|---|
STAI | Absence | 27 | 43.07 (11.77) | 0.004 |
Presence | 31 | 51.80 (10.21) | ||
GSES | Absence | 27 | 10.26 ( 3.53) | <0.001 |
Presence | 31 | 6.42 ( 3.79) | ||
ECSA (inhibitory control) | Absence | 27 | 32.33 ( 4.82) | 0.001 |
Presence | 31 | 27.74 ( 4.89) | ||
ECSA (activation control) | Absence | 27 | 34.96 ( 7.43) | 0.004 |
Presence | 31 | 28.94 ( 8.04) | ||
Duration of gambling (years) | Absence | 27 | 26.59 ( 7.75) | 0.005 |
Presence | 31 | 19.16 (11.25) | ||
Age (years) | Absence | 27 | 50.55 ( 9.70) | 0.001 |
Presence | 31 | 40.90 (11.60) | ||
N | Median (range) | Pb | ||
ECSA (attentional control) | Absence | 27 | 32 (2-48) | 0.024 |
Presence | 31 | 28 (1-43) | ||
Number of months of participation in GA | Absence | 27 | 33 (1-138) | 0.839 |
Presence | 31 | 32 (1-121) | ||
Debt (ten thousand yen) | Absence | 27 | 400 (0-6000) | 0.574 |
Presence | 31 | 400 (0-1300) |
Table 2: Factors associated with the presence or absence of repeated gambling –univariate analysis. a: t-test, b: Mann-Whitney U-test
Factors associated with the presence or absence of repeated gambling -multivariate analysis-
A logistic regression analysis was performed with the presence or absence of repeated gambling as the dependent variable and 8 items showing significant differences in the univariate analysis as explanatory variables. As a result, two items, GSES (P = 0.029) and ECSA (inhibitory control) (P = 0.049), were identified as significant factors (Table 3).
Estimate (beta) | Standard error | Odds ratio | 95% confidence interval | P | VIF | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
STAI | 0.01 | 0.06 | 1.02 | 0.90-1.14 | 0.828 | 1.626 |
GSES | -0.31 | 0.14 | 0.72 | 0.55-0.96 | 0.029 | 2.257 |
ECSA (inhibitory control) | -0.24 | 0.12 | 0.78 | 0.62-0.99 | 0.049 | 2.984 |
ECSA (activation control) | 0.04 | 0.08 | 1.04 | 0.89-1.23 | 0.572 | 3.057 |
ECSA (attentional control) | 0.13 | 0.10 | 1.14 | 0.94-1.37 | 0.18 | 2.641 |
Duration of gambling | 0.06 | 0.07 | 1.06 | 0.93-1.22 | 0.364 | 4.370 |
Age | -0.10 | 0.06 | 0.89 | 0.80-1.02 | 0.098 | 4.092 |
Table 3: Factors associated with the presence or absence of repeated gambling -multivariate analysis. VIF: Variance inflation factors
The male-to-female ratio of gambling disorder is approximately 6:1 (86% in male and 14% in female) in Japan (Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare), and the number of female who participates in GA is less than that that of male. Therefore, female participants in this study were also too small, and we could not assess the differences in characteristics between male and female with gambling disorder.
The multivariate analysis identified two factors associated with repeated gambling, i.e., GSES (self-efficacy) and ECSA (inhibitory control). Self-efficacy is “the expectation of whether one can properly perform an action,” an antecedent factor of an action, and a cognitive variable determining a person’s behavior [9]. The results of this study revealed that self-efficacy was significantly higher in the non-repeated gambling group than in the repeated gambling group. “Modeling” and “successful experience” are said to be effective factors that increase self-efficacy [15]. Namely, it is easily imaginable that members who began to participate in GA could not control their desire for gambling, had social problems such as false words and actions and multiple debts, were not trusted by their families or people at their workplaces, and lived with the loss of confidence and anticipatory anxiety. Those who did not gamble again were likely to begin to expect that “they might be able to stop gambling” by encountering “senior members” (models) in GA meetings, who had similar experiences but continuously quitted gambling, thereby getting rid of anticipatory anxiety and increasing self-efficacy. Furthermore, it is also suggested that these members increased self-affirmation and self-efficacy by repeated “successful experiences,” while imitating (modeling) the method to overcome gambling disorder. Increased self-efficacy seemed to allow them to perform appropriate coping behavior and problem-solving behavior even under impulsive desire for gambling.
On the other hand, however, not all participants could continuously quit repeated gambling after participating in GA, and 56.9% of the participants gambled again. Approximately half of these participants (29.3% of all participants) gambled again once or twice, but 15.5% were habitual and gambled again 10 times or more. These subjects in the repeated gambling group might originally have had low self-efficacy and had not changed after participation, resulting in repeated gambling. However, it could not be determined whether this was the case from this survey alone. Even if these subjects gamble repeatedly, continuous participation in GA may reduce the possibility of having social problems such as multiple debts like before, and allow them to increase self-efficacy through “successful experience.
The Effortful Control (EC), which is a conceptual scale of individual differences in attentional control ability [13], is thought to be associated not only with the control of approach/avoidance behavior and attention but also with the control of emotional experience and expression [12]. For example, inhibitory control prevents sad emotions due to painful experiences from leading to impulsive behavior. Inhibitory control in EC is the “capacity to suppress inappropriate approach behavior,” [13] and this can be said to be the “ability to control oneself without approaching things that must not be done.” This is the ability to actively and intentionally control behavior and control thoughts and emotions with subjective feelings through executive attention according to the demands of the situation [16], unlike a similar system, Gray’s behavior control system, i.e., “passive and automatic behavioral suppression as a cue for punishment.”
The results of this study revealed that inhibitory control of EC was significantly lower in the repeated gambling group than in the non-repeated gambling group. Cheetham et al. [17] reported that drug addicts were unable to escape from unpleasant feelings or overcome the impact of strong feelings, resulting in inappropriate responses to distress such as continuous pathological addiction, and furthermore that low levels of EC may affect substance and behavioral addictions, thereby impairing various abilities to control behavior. Pathological gamblers may have similar responses, because it has been reported that brain activation in pathological gamblers is very similar to that in drug addicts [18,19]. These findings suggest that the subjects in the repeated gambling group could not control behavior, resulting in repeated gambling, and it can be said that our findings were similar to those of previous studies.
There are some limitations in this study. First, this study involved GA groups only in a particular district and the sample size was small, limiting the generalization of the results. For generalizing the results, it is necessary to conduct a nationwide survey in the future. Second, in this study, the questionnaires contained no items concerning family background. It is also necessary to conduct a survey of family background, because it has been reported that the higher the family’s recognition of emotional support networks, the higher the self-esteem and problem-solving behavioral traits, indicating that family background greatly affects the disease condition [20]. Likewise, other variables that may be related to gambling, for example, level of gambling addiction, time spent gambling, or other personality and health-related factors, should have been assessed. Third, for elucidating factors associated with continuous abstinence from gambling after participation in GA, it is necessary to conduct time-series and longitudinal surveys and studies of the timing of participation, level of achievement of program tasks, evaluation of psychological scales, and so on. Finally, the sample size was too low to analyze the data using multivariate analysis. Therefore, the results in the present study are tentative.
The results of this study revealed that approximately 60% of the subjects gambled again after participating in GA, indicating the difficulty in suppressing impulsive gambling addiction. Self-efficacy and the Effortful Control (inhibitory control) were significantly associated with the presence or absence of repeated gambling after participation in GA. These results suggested that it may be possible to identify people who have a temperament that makes them tend to gamble again by evaluating their self-efficacy and inhibitory control. However, the present study was conducted by a cross-sectional design. Therefore, it is necessary to clarify causal relationship between repeated gambling and self-efficacy or inhibitory control in future larger surveys.
Make the best use of Scientific Research and information from our 700 + peer reviewed, Open Access Journals