ISSN: 2375-4494

Journal of Child and Adolescent Behavior
Open Access

Our Group organises 3000+ Global Conferenceseries Events every year across USA, Europe & Asia with support from 1000 more scientific Societies and Publishes 700+ Open Access Journals which contains over 50000 eminent personalities, reputed scientists as editorial board members.

Open Access Journals gaining more Readers and Citations
700 Journals and 15,000,000 Readers Each Journal is getting 25,000+ Readers

This Readership is 10 times more when compared to other Subscription Journals (Source: Google Analytics)
  • Commentary   
  • J Child Adolesc Behav, Vol 9(6)

Should we View Autism More Positively?

Hannah Pearson, Barry Wright and Penny Spikins*
1School of Education, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom
2Department of Health Sciences, University of York, York, United Kingdom
3Department of Archaeology, University of York, York, United Kingdom
*Corresponding Author: Penny Spikins, Department of Archaeology, University of York, York YO10, United Kingdom, Tel: + 441904323962, Email: penny.spikins@york.ac.uk

Received: 18-Oct-2021 / Accepted Date: 01-Nov-2021 / Published Date: 08-Nov-2021

Keywords:  

About the Study

Recent research has shown that the terminology used for describing autism is important to those who are being described [1]. In an Australian study 198 adults, when asked, preferred terms such as ‘autistic person’ or ‘person on the autism spectrum’. In the UK autistic adults preferred the term ‘autistic’ although there was no clear consensus about the best terms to use and autistic people and professionals have differing views [2] suggesting that we should be mindful to ask individual people or groups their preferences. More importantly several themes emerged from the Australian study: That autistic people regarded autism as reflecting diversity (and part of their identity) and struggled with over medicalised language and rejected stigmatising language. Organisations on the internet giving information about autism can present that information as difference but many continue to use medical language and describe deficit and impairment [3].

Deficit focused language dominates discussion around autism in many different areas. For example, diagnostic systems use terminology such as ‘failure’ and ‘abnormalities’ (APA, 2013). Awareness of autism is high in the UK [4] and other western countries [5,6] however misconceptions still exist such as confusing autism with intellectual disability [4]. Awareness of autism among people from ethnic minorities in the UK is much lower [4]. The medicalised approach to autism implemented through diagnostic systems may not promote acceptance of the condition; A desire to cure autism among individuals is heavily associated with stigma around autism [7]. Media representations of autism are also often deficit focused, with terms such as ‘suffering’ and ‘victim’ frequently used [8]. Recent media coverage of an early intervention study for autism [9], has been reported by newspapers in a way which could suggest that poor parenting is a factor in the condition [10] despite this not being implied in the original paper.

A light was shone on this issue by our recent paper suggesting that autism could and should be described in a more positive way by society and certainly by those in statutory services such as health services [11]. This explores things that autistic people are good at and the positive things they bring to society. For example, research has found autistic people to have advanced skills in memory [12], mathematics [13], visual [14,15] and sensory perception [12], music [16], art [17,18] and pattern/system comprehension [19,20]. In many areas the skills of people with autism are not dissimilar of those without [21,22] so how then can we justify this deficit-based description of autism that is currently so prevalent in our society?

This paper goes one step further and asks what the evidence is in deeper historical times or in prehistory for the presence and influence of autistic people. Whilst we need to treat such evidence with caution, there is considerable cause to consider that autistic people had much to offer. Indeed, taking an evolutionary perspective helps us understand some of the important contributions that autistic people make in society. For example, attention to detail and logical thinking may have been beneficial skills for hunter gatherers to have in prehistoric times [23]. Arguably, these skills are also important for humans today: A logical thinking approach with reduced influence of emotionality may be advantageous to have when working within justice and legal systems [24]. In this way terminology used to describe autistic people should usefully describe strengths as well as differences [19,24].

One way of achieving this could be through reframing diagnostic systems in a more balanced way that describes difference rather than deficits. The below gives an example of how this could be constructed.

Differences in social communication compared with neuro-typical people such as

• Logical approach to appraisal of socio-emotional situations.

• Utilitarian approach to the need for communication.

• Preference for communicating only when it is necessary to achieve an outcome (often using written or electronic communication in preference to verbal and nonverbal communication).

• Stronger reliance on environmental information than eye contact and body language.

• Small close group of functional relationships in preference to larger group of social acquaintances

Differences in patterns of interest and occupation, as manifested by

• Liking for structure and routine.

• A tendency to an interest in facts, details, categorization, patterns, visual or topographical memory, numeracy and how things work.

• Differences in interaction with the sensory environment including ability to perceive patterns and details that others can’t easily perceive.

• A tendency to like rules and logic.

These are an alternative set of criteria for autism spectrum disorder.

This paper highlights the need for a difference-based approach to describing autism and begins to address ways in which this could be achieved. Further action is needed to implement a more positive description of autism into diagnostic systems, media coverage and public society.

References

  1. Bury, Simon, Rachel Jellett, Jennifer Spoor and Darren Hedley. "It Defines Who I Am or It’s Something I Have: What Language do (Autistic) Australian Adults (on The Autism Spectrum) Prefer?" J Autism Devel Dis (2020): 1-11.
  2. Kenny, Lorcan, Caroline Hattersley, Bonnie Molins and Elizabeth Pellicano, et al. "Which Terms Should be used to Describe Autism? Perspectives from the UK Autism Community." Autism 20 (2016): 442-462.
  3. Leatherland, Julia and Nick Chown. "What Is Autism? A Content Analysis of Online Autism Information." Good Autism Pract 16 (2015): 27-41.
  4. Dillenburger, Karola, Julie Ann Jordan, Lyn McKerr and Mickey Keenan, et al. "Awareness and Knowledge of Autism and Autism Interventions: A General Population Survey." Res Autism Spec Dis 7 (2013): 1558-1567.
  5. DeVilbiss, Elizabeth and Brian Lee. "Brief Report: Trends in US National Autism Awareness from 2004 to 2014: The Impact of National Autism Awareness Month." J Autism Devel Dis 44 (2014): 3271-3273.
  6. Durand-Zaleski, Isabelle, Jan Scott, Frédéric Rouillon and Marion Leboyer. "A First National Survey of Knowledge, Attitudes and Behaviours towards Schizophrenia, Bipolar Disorders and Autism in France." BMC Psych 12 (2012): 1-8.
  7. Gillespie-Lynch, Kristen, Steven Kapp, Patricia Brooks and Ben Schwartzman. "Whose Expertise Is It? Evidence for Autistic Adults as Critical Autism Experts." Front Psychol 8 (2017): 438.
  8. Huws, Jaci, and Robert Jones. "Missing Voices: Representations of Autism in British Newspapers, 1999-2008." Brit J Learn Disab 39 (2011): 98-104.
  9. Whitehouse, Andrew, Kandice Varcin, Sarah Pillar and Catherine Bent et al. "Effect of Preemptive Intervention on Developmental Outcomes among Infants Showing Early Signs of Autism: A Randomized Clinical Trial of Outcomes to Diagnosis." JAMA Pediatrics (2021): e213298-e213298.
  10. Knapton. “Coaching Parents Found To Reduce Autism Diagnosis by Two-Thirds.” The Telegraph (2021).
  11. Wright, Barry, Penny Spikins and Hannah Pearson. "Should Autism Spectrum Conditions Be Characterised in a More Positive Way in our Modern World?" Medicina (2020): 233.
  12. Howlin, Patricia, Susan Goode, Jane Hutton and Michael Rutter. "Savant Skills in Autism: Psychometric Approaches and Parental Reports." Philos Trans Biol Scie 364 (2009): 1359-1367.
  13. Iuculano, Teresa, Miriam Rosenberg-Lee, Kaustubh Supekar and Vinod Menon, et al. "Brain Organization Underlying Superior Mathematical Abilities in Children with Autism." Biol Psych 75 (2014): 223-230.
  14. Blaser, Erik, Luke Eglington, Alice Carter and Zsuzsa Kaldy. "Pupillometry Reveals A Mechanism for the Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Advantage in Visual Tasks." Scien Rep 4 (2014): 1-5.
  15. Kaldy, Zsuzsa, Ivy Giserman, Alice Carter and Erik Blaser. "The Mechanisms Underlying the ASD Advantage in Visual Search." J Autism Devel Dis 46 (2016): 1513-1527.
  16. Heaton, Pamela. "Assessing Musical Skills in Autistic Children Who are not Savants." Philos Trans Biol Scie 364 (2009): 1443-1447.
  17. Buck, Lucien, Elayne Kardeman and Fran Goldstein. "Artistic Talent in Autistic Adolescents and Young Adults." Emp Stud Arts 3 (1985): 81-104.
  18. Wright, Barry, Hannah Pearson, Penny Spikins and John Schofield et al. "Autism and Engagement with Material Culture." Interdisc Scie Rev (2021): 1-22.
  19. “A. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders.” Amer Psych Ass 3 (1980).
  20. Baron-Cohen, Simon, Ashwin Emma, Ashwin Chris and Teresa Tavassoli, et al. "Talent in Autism: Hyper-Systemizing, Hyper-Attention to Detail and Sensory Hypersensitivity." Philos Transact Biol Scie 364 (2009): 1377-1383.
  21. Kirchner, Jennifer, Willibald Ruch and Isabel Dziobek. "Brief Report: Character Strengths in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder without Intellectual Impairment." J Autism Devel Dis 46 (2016): 3330-3337.
  22. Lorenz, Timo and Kathrin Heinitz. "Aspergers-Different, Not Less: Occupational Strengths and Job Interests of Individuals with Asperger's Syndrome." PloS one 9 (2014): e100358.
  23. Spikins, Penny and Barry Wright. "The Prehistory of Autism." Rounded Globe (2016).
  24. Lei, Jiedi, Lauren Jones and Mark Brosnan. "Exploring an E-Learning Community’s Response to the Language and Terminology Use in Autism from Two Massive Open Online Courses on Autism Education and Technology Use." Autism (2021): 1362361320987963.

Citation: Pearson, Hannah, Wright Barry, Spikins Penny. “Should we View Autism More Positively?” J Child Adolesc Behav 9 (2021): 422.

Copyright: © 2021 Spikins P, et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Top