Poverty, Development and the Brain
Received: 05-Nov-2017 / Accepted Date: 14-Nov-2017 / Published Date: 16-Nov-2017 DOI: 10.4172/2314-7326.1000269
Abstract
Poverty is not an easily defined entity. Many studies have used low Socio-Economic Status (SES) as a surrogate to study its effects. Children in poverty exist in both developing and developed countries. In the United States in 2014, 21% of all children younger than 18 years (15.5 million) lived in “poor” households (i.e. with incomes below 100% of the Federal Poverty Line).
Description
Poverty is not an easily defined entity. Many studies have used low Socio-Economic Status (SES) as a surrogate to study its effects. Children in poverty exist in both developing and developed countries. In the United States in 2014, 21% of all children younger than 18 years (15.5 million) lived in “poor” households (i.e. with incomes below 100% of the Federal Poverty Line [1].
Cooper and Stewart showed that poorer children have worse cognitive, socio-behavioural and health outcomes not just because poverty is correlated with other household and parental characteristics, but in part because they are poorer, because money makes a difference to children’s outcomes [2]. The authors showed that effect sizes associated with a US$1000 increase in income (in 2013) ranged from 5% to 27% of a standard deviation for cognitive outcomes and from 9% to 24% for social and behavioural outcomes. Sirin et al. in a meta-analysis of socio-economic status and academic achievement, showed that the parent’s location in the socio-economic structure has a strong impact on students academic achievement and that family SES at the student level is one of the strongest correlates of academic performance [3].
Studies have shown that poverty affects children in 4 main ways: language and reading, memory, executive functioning (EF) and socio-emotional processing, all factors critical for school success.
Language is a key life skill; significant impairment has long lasting effects for both the child and the adult. Language development also depends on memory and executive functioning, which are both affected by poverty. Perkins et al. in a review of poverty and language, showed the effect on vocabulary, phonological awareness and syntax and reported that there was almost a full standard deviation of difference between the language of children from high and low SES [4]. Hart and Risley showed that word usage in the home was the single strongest determinant of child vocabulary growth, and that low language complexity in lower income homes was a major predictor of vocabulary growth in children [5]. By 26 months, the highest SES children had double the word types of those in the lowest SES. Ursache et al. on reviewing the studies on reading, suggested that among children at risk of reading difficulties, those from higher SES families may recruit alternate, possibly complementary neural networks to develop better reading skills [6]. High SES homes have rich Home Literacy Environments (HLEs, a composite score of literacy activities, maternal engagement and learning materials), with the reverse true in low SES homes [4]. 70% of children with high stable HLEs performed at or above the national norms for language and literacy skills at Pre-kindergarten compared to only 7% of those with low HLE scores [7].
The Centre for the Developing Child has reported that there is a set of underlying core capabilities that adults use to manage life, work, and parenting effectively [8]. These include, but are not limited to: planning, focus, self-control, awareness, and flexibility. These come under the umbrella of EF skills. These skills are supported by the prefrontal cortex and are “as important for early success in school as general intelligence” [9]. Lawson et al. in their meta-analysis of SES and EF in children showed the presence of SES disparities in EF that were between small and medium in size [10] Children’s EF skills have been shown to be robustly predicted by chronic exposure to poverty and to environmental hazards associated with poverty [11]. Low SES tends to be associated with worse performance on memory tasks, and individuals of higher SES are reported to recruit additional neural resources, which may buffer age-related decline [6]. Among middle school youth, low income predicted learned helplessness, significantly affected youths’ self-report of psychological distress and teachers’ ratings of self-regulatory behaviour [12].
There are now numerous studies demonstrating the differences in brain structure and function in areas between children from high and low SES. Noble et al. showed that family income was significantly associated with total surface area, with the strongest relationships in bilateral inferior temporal, insula and inferior frontal gyrus, and in the right occipital and medial prefrontal cortex–regions linked with various language and executive functions [13]. Emotional processing and cognitive control over emotion which are central to self-regulation are affected by the development of the amygdala, hippocampus and PFC which work together to regulate emotion, which is also important for language development. Hanson et al. have shown the relationship between income and total hippocampal gray matter, an area important for learning and memory [14]. Kim et al. showed that adults with lower family income at age 9 had reduced ventrolateral and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity and failure to suppress amygdala activation at age 24 [15]. Both areas are important for stress and emotional regulation. Poor cognitive and academic performance among children in poverty has been shown to be mediated by a small hippocampus and frontal and temporal lobes [16]. The decrease in volume of the latter 2 explained as much as 15% to 20% of the achievement deficits found. Children from low income families have also been shown to have slower trajectories of growth during infancy and childhood, with volumetric difference associated with the emergence of disruptive behavioural problems [17]. They also had lower volumes of gray matter (tissue critical for the processing of information and execution of actions) in both the frontal and parietal lobes.
Noble et al. have showed that this relationship between poverty and the brain is not linear but logarithmic. Income related most strongly to brain structure among the most disadvantaged children; for every dollar increase in increased income, the increase in children’s brain surface area was proportionally greater at the lower end of the spectrum [13]. The proportion of IQ variance attributable to genes and environment has also been shown to vary non-linearly with SES [18].
The effect on brain function has been seen in early infancy. Tomalski et al. found socioeconomic disparities in frontal gamma power in infants as young as 6-9 months, pointing to very early risk for language and attention difficulties [19]. Gao et al reported that marginally significant positive SES-brain correlation was observed at 6 months of age for both the sensorimotor and default-mode networks, indicating SES effects on functional brain maturation [20].
Lipina summarized the available literature in 3 statements [21]:
• The different experiences of adversity related to poverty are associated with changes in the structure and function of neural systems that are related to cognitive and emotional regulation, language and learning skills. The authors cautioned against concluding that changes represent deficits or dysfunctions, that such findings are irreversible or that correlational evidence equalled causality.
• These influences could occur at different times during human development.
• The hypothetical mechanisms through which these changes occur involve different factors that are related to childcare through the quality of language environments and through cognitive stimulation and emotional support at home and in education contexts.
Can the trajectory of these children in adversity be changed so that they have a better chance of success? There have been numerous programs to reduce the adverse outcome of poverty on children, which have shown various positive effects. High quality care giving is a key strategy; it can “serve as a key lever of change through which effects of disadvantageous experience on biology and behaviour can be altered” [22]. Bann et al. showed that a home-based parent implemented early developmental intervention from 1-36 months of age resulted in significantly higher scores in the Mental Development Index than the control group [23]. Several early childhood programs have reported good success. The Chicago School Readiness Project, a multicomponent trial implemented in 35 Head Start centers suggested significant benefits on children’s pre-academic skills, as measured by vocabulary, letter-naming and maths skills [24]. The Perry School Project was a 2-year preschool intervention for disadvantaged children. At 40 years of age, the treated group had higher rates of high school graduation, higher salaries, higher percentages of home ownership, lower rates of welfare assistance as adults, fewer out of wedlock births and fewer arrests than controls [25]. The children in the Abecedarian project in North Carolina, a randomized controlled trial of early childhood education for children from low-income families were followed up at 30 years of age [26]. There were significant and moderately large educational gains, with treated individuals having 13.46 years of education and compared to 12.31 years in the controls. 23% had Bachelor’s degrees compared to 6% of the controls. 75% worked full time compared to 53% of the controls. The odds of being the head of one’s own household were almost twice as high for the treated group. The probability of needed public welfare in the control group was more than 6 times that of the treated group. Other programs have shown various levels of success. These include the early identification of families in need of services, home visiting, nutrition support, early childhood education, access to comprehensive health care and tax policies and direct financial aid (from poverty and child health in the USA) [1]. Bringings all the critical components together requires coordinated public policy measures, which is a huge challenge. However, to bridge the achievement gap between the children from high and low SES and possibly to protect the next generation from the same adversity, it is a public health step that needs to be taken.
References
- American Academy of Pediatrics (2015) Poverty and child health in the United States. Pediatrics 137: e20160339.
- Cooper K, Stewart K (2013) Does money affect children’s outcomes? A systematic review. Joseph Rowntree Foundation, UK.
- Sirin SR (2005) Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Rev Ed Res 27: 417–453.Â
- Perkins SC, Finegood ED, Swain JE (2013) Poverty and language: Roles of parenting and stress. Innov Clin Neurosci 10: 10-19.
- Hart B, Risley TR (1995) Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H Brookes Publishing, USA.
- Uarsche A, Noble KG (2016) Neurocognitive development in socio-economic context: Multiple mechanisms and implications of measuring socioeconomic status. Psychophysiology 53: 71-82.
- Rodriquez ET, Tamis-Lena CT (2011) Trajectories of the home learning environment across the first 5 years: Associations with children’s vocabulary and literacy at prekindergarten. Child Dev 82: 1058-1075.
- Center for the Developing Child. Three principles to improve outcomes for children and families. Harvard University, USA.
- Blair C, Raver CC (2015) School readiness and self-regulation: A developmental psychobiological approach. Ann Rev Psychol 66: 711-731.
- Lawson GM, Hook CJ, Farah MJ (2017) A meta-analysis of the relationship between socioeconomic status and executive function performance among children. Dev Sci.
- Raver CC, Blair C, Willoughby M (2012) Poverty as a predictor of 4-year-olds' executive function: new perspectives on models of differential susceptibility. Dev Psychol 49: 292-304.
- Evans GW, Gonella C, Marcynyszyn LA, Gentile L, Salpekar N (2015) The role of chaos in poverty and children’s socioemotional adjustment. Psychol Sci 16: 560-565.
- Noble KG, Houston SM, Brito NH, Bartsch H, Kan E, et al. (2015) Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nat Neurosci 18: 773-778.
- Hanson JL, Chandra A, Wolfe BB, Pollak SD (2011) Association between income and the Hippocampus. Plos One 6: e18172.
- Kim P, Evans GW, Angstadt M, Ho SH, Sripada CS, et al. (2013) Effects of childhood poverty and chronic stress on emotion regulatory brain function in adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110: 18442-18447.
- Hair NL, Hanson JL, Wolfe BL (2105) Association of child poverty, brain development and academic achievement. JAMA Pediatr 169: 822-829.
- Hanson JL, Hair N, Shen DG, Shi F, Gilmore JH, et al. (2013) Family poverty affects the rate of human brain growth. PLOS One 8: e80954.
- Turkheimer E, Haley A, Waldron M, D’Onofrio B, Gottesman II (2003) Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychol Sci 14: 623-628.
- Tomalski P, Moore DG, Riberio H, Axelsson E, Murphy E, et al. (2103) Socioeconomic status and functional brain development-associations in early infancy. Dev Sci 16: 676-687.
- Gao W, Alcauter S, Elton A, Hernandez-Castillo CR, Smith JK, et al. (2015) Functional network development during the first year: Relative sequence and socio-economic correlations. Cereb Cortex 25: 2919-2928.
- Lipina SJ, Evers K (2017) Neuroscience of childhood poverty: Evidence of impacts and mechanisms as vehicles of dialog with ethics. Front Psychol 8: 61.
- Blair C, Raver CC (2016) Poverty, stress and brain development: New directions for prevention and intervention. Acad Pediatr 16: 30-36.
- Bann CM, Wallender JL, Do Barbara, Thorsten V, Pasha Omrana FJ, et al. (2016) Home based early intervention and the influence of family resources on cognitive development. Pediatrics 137: 1-9.
- Raver CC, Jones SM, Li-Grining C, Zhai F, Bub K, et al. (2011) CSRP’s impact on low-income preschoolers’ preacademic skills: Self-regulation as a mediating mechanism. Child Dev 82: 362-378.
- Heckman J (2006) Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged children. Science 312: 1900-1902.
- Campbell FA, Pugello EP, Kainz K, Burchinal M, Yi P, et al. (2012) Young adult outcomes from the abecedarian and CARE early Childhood educational interventions. Dev Pyschol 48: 1033-1043.
Citation: Daniel LM (2017) Poverty, Development and the Brain. J Neuroinfect Dis 8:269 DOI: 10.4172/2314-7326.1000269
Copyright: ©2017 Daniel LM. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Share This Article
Recommended Journals
Open Access Journals
Article Tools
Article Usage
- Total views: 5307
- [From(publication date): 0-2017 - Dec 22, 2024]
- Breakdown by view type
- HTML page views: 4538
- PDF downloads: 769