Dersleri yüzünden oldukça stresli bir ruh haline sikiş hikayeleri bürünüp özel matematik dersinden önce rahatlayabilmek için amatör pornolar kendisini yatak odasına kapatan genç adam telefonundan porno resimleri açtığı porno filmini keyifle seyir ederek yatağını mobil porno okşar ruh dinlendirici olduğunu iddia ettikleri özel sex resim bir masaj salonunda çalışan genç masör hem sağlık hem de huzur sikiş için gelip masaj yaptıracak olan kadını gördüğünde porn nutku tutulur tüm gün boyu seksi lezbiyenleri sikiş dikizleyerek onları en savunmasız anlarında fotoğraflayan azılı erkek lavaboya geçerek fotoğraflara bakıp koca yarağını keyifle okşamaya başlar
Reach Us +44 1223 790975

GET THE APP

Neural Substrates of Reserve Observed in a Non-Demented Aging Population | OMICS International
ISSN: 2161-0460
Journal of Alzheimers Disease & Parkinsonism

Like us on:

Make the best use of Scientific Research and information from our 700+ peer reviewed, Open Access Journals that operates with the help of 50,000+ Editorial Board Members and esteemed reviewers and 1000+ Scientific associations in Medical, Clinical, Pharmaceutical, Engineering, Technology and Management Fields.

Neural Substrates of Reserve Observed in a Non-Demented Aging Population

Jeanyung Chey1,2*, M Justin Kim3, Yaakov Stern2, Minyoung Shin1, Hong-Sik Byun4and Christian Habeck2

1Department of Psychology and Interdisciplinary Program for Brain Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul 151-746, South Korea

2Cognitive Neuroscience Division, Department of Neurology, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York, NY 10032, USA

3Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Moore Hall, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH 03755, USA

4Department of Radiology, Samsung Medical Center, Sungkyunkwan University School of Medicine, Seoul 135-710, South Korea

Corresponding Author:
Jeanyung Chey
Department of Psychology
Seoul National University, Building 16
Gwanakro 599, Seoul 151-746, South Korea
Tel: +822-880-6432
E-mail: jychey@snu.ac.kr

Received date: December 07, 2016; Accepted date: December 26, 2016; Published date: December 31, 2016

Citation: Chey J, Kim MJ, Stern Y, Shin M, Byun HS, et al. (2016) Neural Substrates of Reserve Observed in a Non-Demented Aging Population. J Alzheimers Dis Parkinsonism 6:294. doi:10.4172/2161-0460.1000294

Copyright: © 2016 Chey J, et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Visit for more related articles at Journal of Alzheimers Disease & Parkinsonism

Abstract

Cognitive decline in dementia does not correspond precisely to the amount of neurodegeneration in the brain. This discrepancy in brain damage and its clinical manifestation has been explicated by the concept of reserve. Brain reserve inferred from the brain size had moderate success in explaining the discrepancy, and numerous studies have reported the effects of education supporting cognitive reserve. Yet the neural substrates of reserve have been elusive. Utilizing optimized voxel-based morphometry, we have identified brain regions that were significantly smaller in individuals with low cognitive performance (LCP) compared to those with normal cognitive performance (NCP) in community-residing non-demented elderly people both with low educational attainment. It was assumed that the cognitive performance in this population reflected long-standing cognitive functioning of the individual, possibly the reserve, based on their stable follow-up performance and clinical interviews. Bilateral precuneus, right superior frontal gyrus and left middle frontal gyrus were smaller in individuals with LCP. Further, the LCP individuals had weaker correlation between the gray matter volume of those regions and the rest of the cortex. On the other hand, volume of these regions was more tightly correlated with the K-DRS Total score in these individuals. Finally, an outcome study of the community sample from which this study's participants were recruited from reported five times higher risk of dementia in the LCP group. Precuneus and prefrontal cortex are proposed as key sites comprising the neural substrates that underlie the reserve.

Keywords

Education; Precuneus; Prefrontal cortex; Structural correlation; Voxel-based morphometry.

Individuals with superior intelligence develop dementia symptoms later in the course of illness and low education has been repeatedly observed as a risk for Alzheimer’s disease and dementia in general [1-3]. It has been proposed that individuals with greater cognitive reserve, measured with proxies such as education, occupation, and the Intelligence Quotient (IQ), can withstand the effects of brain injury or neurodegenerative processes and delay the clinical manifestation of dementia, while individuals with smaller reserve would manifest the symptoms earlier in the disease process [2,4,5].

Reserve refers to the individual’s ability to tolerate the effect of brain disease, and delay or even prevent the development of clinical symptoms of neuropathology [2,5]. Initially, reserve was conceptualized in simple brain measures of integrity, such as the whole brain volume and synaptic density [1,6], characterized as brain reserve [2]. Conceptualized it cognitively as an active a cognitive process that enables the individual to cope with after brain damage or neurodegeneration. Consistent with the reserve theory, education, the most popular proxy, has been repeatedly identified as a moderator in the development of dementia in longitudinal outcome studies. While low educational attainment has been found to be a significant risk factor for dementia [7-9], greater years of education has been shown to delay the clinical manifestation of dementia in proportion to the number of years [4,10]. These findings were confirmed and replicated in post-mortem neuropathology [11-13] as well as neuroimaging studies [14-17]. Occupation, intelligence and more recently leisure activities, other proxies of cognitive reserve, have also shown protective effects against dementia [5].

Recently, attempts to investigate the neural mechanism underlying cognitive reserve have been reported. A study found a common neural network involving the superior and medial frontal gyri that was activated as the processing load increased in a delayed item-recognition task. Based on the assumption that cognitive reserve would share brain network activated during demanding processing load, the results suggest that this network may be consistent with a neural representation of the cognitive reserve [18].

Brain maintenance during aging is another factor that could account for individual differences in dementia risk [19] and studies have shown that elderly individuals with minimal cognitive aging tended to have less senescent or pathological changes in their brain. Prefrontal cortex (PFC), the brain region showing earliest signs of aging, has been studied extensively to examine brain maintenance. For instance, PFC has been found to be thicker in elderly people who performed better in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, a classical test for higher cognitive functioning [20]. Further, successful brain maintenance in this region was manifested in high-performing elderly people who recruited cortical networks similar to young subjects during a working memory task [21,22].

Compared to the neural correlates of cognitive reserve or the brain maintenance factors, the features of brain reserve have been elaborated in scant detail. The earlier theories of brain reserve [1,6] focusing on whole brain volume or the synaptic density were effective in explaining the reserve effect by utilizing a quantitative threshold [23] and studies have demonstrated that gross brain volumes and IQ, a key proxy of the reserve, were correlated in young [24] as well as old adults [25]. The brain size, however, is limited in accounting for the individual difference in cognitive processing that could make a significant difference within similar brain-sized people [2]. And it has been shown that fMRI activation patterns during a visual encoding task correlated better with the proxies such as IQ, education, occupation, and leisureactivities [26] than the whole brain volume. Further, brain reserve has been considered a passive model due to its assumption of a threshold at which clinical symptoms manifest without consideration of individual differences in how brain processes cognitive tasks [2]. While this may have been a valid point, recent studies have found evidence that brain structures as well as functions change over the adult lifespan due to developmental [27,28], environmental [29,30] and medical factors [31]. Further, whole brain volume is a single index that does not reflect individual differences in regional structures. Since recent structural imaging techniques provides voxel counts, volume or thickness of the regions, the brain reserve concept can be approached anew using these regional measures, and shed light into the neural substrates of cognitive differences in elderly people. Studies have investigated the relation between intelligence and regional brain measures mostly in younger adults, and found cortical regions, mostly the prefrontal cortex and the parietal lobe [32] to be associated with IQ. Yet, few studies have explored the regional brain volumes that are associated with cognitive ability or proxies of reserve in the context of aging.

In this study, we attempted to find brain regions that were associated with overall cognitive functioning in non-demented communityresiding elderly people. We analyzed the regional gray matter volume of 13 individuals with low cognitive performance (LCP) and compared them to those with 13 normal cognitive performance (NCP) who was matched in age and education. They were all recruited from a sample of 243 non-demented community-residing elderly Koreans who were followed-up longitudinally for 6 years and had been evaluated for dementia diagnosis [33]. Since the cognitive performance in this sample remained stable and did not decline during the following four years, it was assumed that cognitive performance in this population at baseline measures individual's long-standing cognitive functioning status.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Twenty six individuals were recruited from a sample of 243 nondemented community-residing elderly people in the Seoul National University Cognitive Aging Study (SNU-CAS) at baseline. Individuals in the SNU-CAS were non-demented and “normal” according to a criterion from a previous study [34]. They 1) were free of memory and cognitive disorders leading to corresponding functional impairments in everyday life; 2) lived independently without difficulty; 3) had no psychiatric and neurological illness; and 4) had no physical conditions that are known to compromise cognitive capacity such as hypertension or diabetes that could not be controlled by medication, thyroid disease or loss of consciousness for more than 20 min. The participants did not have severe hearing or vision problems that could significantly compromise the neuropsychological test results.

A battery of neuropsychological tests, clinical tests, and interviews were administered to assess the cognitive and behavioural functions that were necessary to ascertain the “normal” status of the elderly participants. Neuropsychological assessment involved 1) a neuropsychological interview with the subject, 2) the Korean version of the Dementia Rating Scale [35,36], 3) the Elderly Memory disorder Scale [37], 4) the Beck Depression Inventory [38], 5) the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [39], 6) the Literacy Questionnaire [40] and a semi-structured interview [41] with an informant to confirm lack of significant change in cognitive and behavioural functions. Further details of the tools are in [42]. Therefore, no one with complaints of cognitive decline either from the subject him/herself or family members was included in the study.

Thirteen individuals had low cognitive performance (LCP) while thirteen had normal cognitive performance (NCP) according to the following criteria. LCP was defined by poor or borderline performance on either the Total score on the K-DRS or the Long-term Memory Recall score on the EMS; i.e., scores less than one standard deviation below the mean compared to the age- and education-specified norms. In order to be classified as having NCP, both the Total K-DRS and the Long-Term Memory Recall of the EMS had to be equal or above the cut-off score.

The two groups were matched on age and years of education, but were significantly different in the rate of literacy (Table 1). The subjects also received baseline brain MRI for voxel-based morphometry. These individuals had no significant findings on their T1 and T2 weighted MRI images that could suggest brain disorders or lesions [42] for detailed description on the exclusionary criteria.

Procedures

At baseline, each participant received an oral briefing on the nature of the study and signed an agreement of participation, followed by a comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation and sampling of the venous blood sample for the determination of the ApoE genotype. Lastly, brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans from thirtyone participants were obtained. Since the institutional review board of Seoul National University was not present during the time of the sevenyear study, researchers abided by the Declaration of Helsinki [43]. The procedural details of the baseline study are described in a previous study [42].

Acquisition and analysis of the magnetic resonance image data

Whole brain magnetic resonance images were acquired on a 1.5-T Signa system (General Electric Medical System, Milwaukee, WI, U.S.A). The MRI protocol included the following: (a) sagittal T1-weighted acquisitions (repetition time (TR) 400 ms, echo time (TE) 11, number of excitations (NEX) 1) with field of view (FOV) of 24 cm, 5-mm slice thickness, and matrix size of 256 × 256; (b) T2-weighted acquisitions (TR 3,000, TE 100, NEX 2) in the coronal planes perpendicular to the long axis of the hippocampus with section thickness of 3 mm, FOV of 21 cm and matrix size of 256 × 256; (c) coronal T1-weighted (TR 30 ms, TE 7, NEX 1, flip angle 45°) three-dimensional (3D) spoiled gradient acquisitions with 124 sections at 1.6 mm slice thickness, FOV of 21 cm and matrix size of 256 × 192.

  LCP (n=13)
Mean+SD or frequency
NCP (n=13)
Mean+SD or frequency
Statistical
Significance
Age 71.38 (8.05) 71.15 (7.87) 0.942
Gender (male/female) 5/8 3/10 0.673
Education 3.00 (4.32) 2.77 (3.44) 0.882
Literacy (n) 7 0 0.005**
K-DRS Total 101.92 (12.09) 128.38 (9.22) 0.000**
EMS DM recall 1.18 (0.56) 1.88 (0.44) 0.002**
Apo e4 allele (n)a 2 0 0.199
Hypertensionb 1/11 2/13 NS
Diabetes 1/13 0/13 NS
LOC (<20 min)c 4/13 1/13 NS
Habitual smoking 1/13 3/13 NS

Table1: Descriptive statistics of the LCP and the NCP participants.

To investigate the group differences in regional gray matter volume, optimized voxel-based-morphometry (VBM) was utilized. We adapted the optimized VBM methods from a previous study [44]. To briefly summarize, data from all 26 participants were used to construct a study-specific template image. Creating a template image tailored to our study was especially important given that typical template images are derived from a young adult sample (e.g. MNI152). Subsequently, the original whole brain images were segmented into gray and white matter images. Unconnected non-brain voxels (i.e., voxels not containing gray matter, white matter, or cerebrospinal fluid) were then extracted from these segmented images [45].

Gray matter images were then spatially normalized again, limiting any effects of non-brain voxels in the normalization process, thereby acquiring optimal spatial normalization of gray matter images. Then, optimized normalization parameters derived from the previous step were reapplied to the original whole brain images. They were spatially normalized and then segmented into gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid. In addition, the Jacobian determinants, derived from spatial normalization, were applied to all gray matter images in order to acquire volume information from the voxels. Finally, all images were smoothed using a Gaussian kernel (full-width–half maximum=8 mm).

Statistical analyses

Independent t-test, chi-square test and analysis of variance (ANOVA) were utilized to tests the group differences on basic demographic, cognitive and health measures and descriptive statistics were calculated.

Voxel-based morphometry

All smoothed images were assigned to either the LCP or the NCP group. Then, the contrasts (LCP<NCP, LCP>NCP) were obtained, using the analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) model, implemented in SPM2 with age, sex and intracranial volume (ICV) as covariates of no interest. ICV was measured with the reconstructed sagittal images of 5 mm thickness SPGR MRI data, details of which are described in [42]. A significance level of p<0.001 (uncorrected) and k=100 voxels was used to report VBM results.

Structural correlation analysis

To examine how relevant brain regions correlate with one another in terms of gray matter density, we employed a seed-based structural correlation analysis by extracting gray matter volume values from the 4 ROIs (“seeds”) identified by the LCP<NCP contrast performed in the aforementioned ANCOVA model. To enable a brain wide analysis of structural correlation we used the AAL-template [46] to convert the structural images of participants into 116 regions of interest (ROIs). To remove the influence of several confounding variables, a multiple regression model that included age, sex and ICV was run on all seed values and on all 116 ROIs across subjects. The residuals of this regression were then correlated in the subsequent analysis. This allowed us to visualize brain regions that correlated with the seed regions across the whole brain while controlling for the effects of age, sex and ICV, hence identifying a structural correlation network for each seed region. Additionally, in order to assess group differences in the structural correlation patterns, we computed each of the 4 structural correlation networks separately for the LCP and the NCP groups and compared the networks with a permutation test (10,000 iterations). Given that there were 4 seeds and 2 groups, a total of 12 structural correlation networks (4 for each group plus 4 for all 26 participants) are reported here.

Regional brain volumes and cognitive functions

To investigate the cognitive functions associated with the four seeds, we performed correlation analysis between the regions and the neuropsychological performance scores, controlling for age, sex and ICV. To further investigate the association of regional brain volumes with overall cognitive functioning, we extended the analysis of the previous section: we used the K-DRS Total score as a “seed” and correlated it with the 116 regional ROIs, again controlling for age, sex, intracranial volume as covariates of no interest. Additionally, in order to assess the LCP versus NCP group differences in the correlation pattern between the K-DRS Total and the brain regions, we compared the correlation pattern with a permutation test (10,000 iterations).

Results

Low cognitive performance and regional gray matter volumes

The LCP group showed significantly smaller gray matter volume in bilateral precuneus and areas in the prefrontal cortex (Figure 1 and Table 2). The most notable difference between the two groups were found in bilateral precuneus, but regions in the right superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and the left middle frontal gyrus (MFG) were also significantly different. Medial temporal lobe structures, such as the hippocampus, did not show differences in gray matter density between the two cognitive performance groups, consistent with the results from the ROI-based volumetry study [42].

Structural correlation networks

Volumes in four areas that were found to be significantly different between the two cognitive groups, were designated as the “seeds,” and further correlation analyses were conducted between the seeds and the rest of the brain regions. It is of note that correlation between these seeds was highly correlated. The correlation between the right and the left precuenus was 0.952, the right precuneus and the right SFG 0.803, the right precuneus and left MFG 0.782, the left precuneus and the right SFG 0.760, the left precuneus and the left MFG.746 and the right SFG and the left MFG 0.663. The coefficients were larger in the NCP group, reaching significant difference in correlations of the left MFG with the right precuneus (p=0.018) and the left precuneus (p=0.031) respectively. The group differences, however, did not survive the multiple-comparison corrections (p=0.05/6).

Brain Regions

MNI Coordinates

Number of
Voxels
LCP (n=13) NCP (n=13) t
x y Z
Precuneus (L) -3 -67 30 2150 0.4709 ± 0.0726 0.5644 ± 0.0976 5.26
Precuneus (R) 6 -75 22 1176 0.3817 ± 0.0708 0.4756 ± 0.0988 5.22
SFG (R) 39 14 55 101 0.2340 ± 0.0279 0.2972 ± 0.0523 5.34
MFG (L) -31 26 38 209 0.2296 ± 0.0318 0.3306 ± 0.0836 5.12

Table 2: Brain regions that were smaller in the LCP individuals compared to the NCP individuals at baseline.

alzheimers-disease-parkinsonism-LCP-group

Figure 1: Brain regions significantly smaller in the LCP group compared to those in the NCP.

The structural correlation analysis between each of the four seeds and the 116 regions of interest (ROIs) were done to identify more comprehensive networks associated with the seeds and yielded four seed-based networks that are depicted in Figure 2. The precuneus-seeded network demonstrated significant correlations with lateral and medial frontal regions as well as medial parietal and occipital regions. Interestingly, the frontal seeds showed similar correlation patterns (Figure 2). We further compared the two cognitive performance groups, and found that correlation coefficients between cortical regions were generally greater in the NCP group (Table 3) for detailed results of the permutation tests.) The areas that showed significant group differences were found mostly in the precuneus-seeded networks. As depicted in Figure 3, the precuneus seeds had significantly higher correlation with the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), the Calcarine cortex, right lingual gyrus and the paracentral lobule in the NCP group compared to the LCP. Similar difference was found with the right SFG-seeded network in the right Calcarine cortex, right lingual gyrus and the cuneus. The left MFG-seeded network showed group difference in the PCC and the left paracentral lobule. The significance of the group difference, however, did not survive the multiple-comparison correction (p<0.05/116).

Brain regions and cognitive functions

Many cognitive scores correlated significantly with the four seeds. As predicted, the total score of the K-DRS correlated significantly with the gray matter density of the areas (Table 4) for the correlation coefficients and top figure of Figure 4. Further, the correlation patterns between the K-DRS Total and the 116 ROIs in the two cognitive groups were investigated to find how brain regions were differently associated with overall cognitive functioning. The K-DRS Total score and the gray matter density of the LCP individuals correlated more strongly than the NCP in the precuneus, the rectus, the vermis, the hippocampus, the frontal cortex, the posterior cingulate cortex and other cerebellar regions as shown in Figure 4. All the correlations were stronger in the LCP group. Scatter plots between the right precuneus, where the difference in correlation was the largest, and the K-DRS Total for both groups are shown in Figure 5.

alzheimers-disease-parkinsonism-Structural-correlation

Figure 2: Structural correlation map between the four seeds and the 116 AAL-ROIs across the combined group of LCP and NCP subjects. Only positive correlations were found at p<0.05.

Precuneus (R)   Precuneus (L)   SFG (R)   MFG (L)  
AAL region   AAL region   AAL region   AAL region  
Mid Frontal, R 0.047 Sup Frontal, L 0.043 Calcarine, R 0.0294 PCC, L 0.015
PCC, L 0.004 IFO, L 0.046 Cuneus, L 0.0292 PCC, R 0.049
PCC, R 0.007 PCC, L 0.014 Cuneus, R 0.0204 Paracentral, L 0.029
Calcarine, L 0.027 PCC, R 0.008 Lingual, R 0.0472    
Calcarine, R 0.006 Calcarine, L 0.025        
Cuneus, L 0.036 Calcarine, R 0.005        
Cuneus, R 0.018 Cuneus, L 0.033        
Lingual, R 0.032 Cuneus, R 0.021        
Sup Occipital, R 0.042 Angular, R 0.011        
Mid Occipital, R 0.040 Precuneus, R 0.025        
Inf Occipital, L 0.038 Paracentral, L 0.023        
Angular, R 0.005 Vermis 10 -0.017        
Precuneus, R 0.034            
Paracentral, L 0.024            
Vermis 10 -0.048            

Table 3: AAL regions that showed significant difference in correlation with the seeds between the two cognitive performance groups.

  Precuneus (L) Precuneus (R) SFG (R) MFG (L)
K-DRS        
Total 0.489* 0.435* 0.451* 0.567**
Attention 0.398 0.427* 0.541** 0.456*
Initiation/Perseveration 0.442* 0.348 0.375 0.462**
Construction 0.044 0.035 -0.021 0.183
Conceptualization 0.353 0.341 0.266 0.510*
Memory 0.432* 0.316 0.322 0.387
EMS        
EMS LTM Recall 0.515* 0.463* 0.466* 0.557**
EVLT        
Learning 0.450* 0.355 0.370 0.513*
S-D Free Recall 0.588** 0.452* 0.429* 0.317
S-D Cued Recall 0.562** 0.458* 0.423* 0.518*
L-D Free Recall 0.425* 0.409 0.453* 0.432*
L-D Cued Recall 0.617** 0.540** 0.534** 0.473*
Recognition 0.264 0.209 0.329 0.261
SRFT        
Copy 0.304 0.368 0.283 0.372
Immediate 0.233 0.311 0.241 0.419*
Delayed 0.290 0.312 0.276 0.431*
Story Recall Test        
Immediate 0.468* 0.383 0.431* 0.429*
Delayed 0.525* 0.429* 0.444* 0.500*
Recognition 0.359 0.259 0.239 0.214
Clock Drawing Test (Total) 0.405 0.351 0.286 0.455*
K-BNT (15 item)*** 0.152 0.093 -0.109 0.054
Go-No-Go Test 0.357 0.292 0.199 0.356

Table 4: Partial correlation coefficients between neuropsychological measures and volumes of the key voxels controlled for age, sex and intracranial volume.

Discussion

Neural substrates of reserve

alzheimers-disease-parkinsonism-gray-matter-density

Figure 3: Significant differences in correlation in the two cognitive performance groups between the gray-matter density of 116 ROIs and right and left precuneus, respectively, as ascertained by a permutation test with 10,000 iterations. Red indicates larger correlations for the NCP, green indicates larger correlations for the LCP, p<0.05.

This study found that non-demented elderly individuals with low cognitive performance (LCP) had significantly smaller gray matter volume or density in bilateral precuneus, the right superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and the left middle frontal gyrus (MFG) compared to those with normal cognitive performance. A subsequent correlation analysis with the four regions used as the seeds was conducted to further investigate the neural substrates underlying cognitive differences, which revealed an overlapping network of cortical regions that consisted of bilateral precuneus, the posterior cingulate cortex, paracentral lobule, the lateral and medial frontal regions, the angular gyrus and the medial and lateral occipital cortex. Moreover, the network differed significantly in its strength of correlation in the two cognitive performance groups; i.e., demonstrating greater correlation in the NCP group in the precuneusseeded networks which primarily consisted of the posterior cingulate cortex, the Calcarine cortex and the paracentral gyrus. Frontal seedednetworks showed the same pattern of greater correlation in the NCP group albeit with less significant regions. Lastly, the K-DRS Total score demonstrated association with the volume or density of the precuneus and this association was stronger in the LCP compared to the NCP group. This pattern of stronger correlation between the K-DRS Total score and the gray matter volume of various brain regions was observed in other brain regions, such as the orbital frontal cortex, the hippocampus, the posterior cingulum and the vermis of cerebellum.

alzheimers-disease-parkinsonism-AAL-brain

Figure 4: AAL brain regions whose gray-matter density correlated significantly with the K-DRS across both LCP and NCP groups (top). Regions that demonstrated significantly higher correlation between the volume and the KDRS Total score in the LCP group compared to the NCP as ascertained by a permutation test with 10,000 iterations. Green indicates larger correlations for the LCP, p<0.05 (bottom)

alzheimers-disease-parkinsonism-right-precuneus

Figure 5: Scatter plots between the right precuneus volume and the K-DRS score in the LCP (r = .666) and the NCP (r= - .412) groups. The correlation coefficients of the two group differed significantly (p = 0.0076.)

Nature of low cognitive performance

It is unlikely that smaller regional gray matter volumes observed in the LCP group was due to mild cognitive impairment or preclinical dementia, since no LCP individuals showed decline, instead demonstrated improvement, in cognitive performance for 4 years following the initial assessment [47]. No atrophy was found in the medial temporal structures of the LCP individuals [42], which are often found to be smaller in individuals with mild cognitive impairment. Lastly, the subject recruitment procedure precluded elderly individuals with complaints of cognitive decline reported either by the individual him/herself or by the family member.

The true nature of lower cognitive performance in these individuals may be elusive, since we do not have access to their childhood and early adult cognitive performance. Nonetheless, several characteristics of the LCP individuals suggest that their cognitive performance reflects a longstanding cognitive functioning status possibly related to intelligence or cognitive reserve. First, a previous study found that intracranial volume (ICV) was significantly smaller in the LCP individuals compared to those of the NCP group [42], demonstrating that their brain reserve was smaller than the normal cognitive functioning individuals. But more importantly, all LCP individuals grew up in remote rural areas where educational opportunity was slim and not required for their livelihood. They were mainly farmers who all received equivalent or less than 6 years of schooling, a few not attending school at all. Since the NCP individuals were matched in terms of years of schooling, however, simple duration of education do not explain their low performance completely. Literacy, one of the basic outcomes of schooling, probably could add more information to the quality of educational experience elderly individuals had. Approximately half of the LCP individuals were functionally illiterate, while everyone in the NCP group was literate despite similar years of schooling. Therefore, the lack of opportunity or ability to learn to read or write could be one of the factors differentiating the two cognitive groups as observed in previous studies [48,49]. Since limited years of education was the necessary condition for all low cognitive performance in our sample, albeit not a sufficient one and literacy an additional dimension, it becomes clear that low cognitive performance in this population reflected life experience that was cognitive in nature. It appears that LCP individuals were not able to develop cognitive functions or brain to their full potential, most likely due to their limited educational experience. This is consistent with the finding that education narrows the variance of cognitive performance, especially boosting the performance of those in the lower strata [50]. We cannot, however, rule out the contribution of gene in cognitive performance. Since the two cognitive groups did not differ significantly in the apo e4 status, other genetic variations may have contributed to the cognitive differences in tandem with their educational experience.

Strength of connectivity and cognitive performance

Structural correlation analysis was able to further specify the neural substrates of cognitive difference in the elderly people; i.e., the reduced correlation in the seeded neural networks of the LCP elderly individuals compared to those of the NCP. The origin of this weaker connectivity or coherence of the cortical regions in LCP may be limited cognitive activities. A recent study examining the effects of education on brain metabolism reported findings supportive of this interpretation, showing that low educational attainment was associated with less functional connectivity especially in the long-distance connections [47], making them more vulnerable to both random and targeted attacks. The diminished cortical connectivity underlying low cognitive performance in the elderly individuals may be similar in nature, possibly making them more vulnerable to aging and neurodegeneration. In fact, elderly people with LCP in the larger community sample have been found to have significantly increased risk, i.e., five times greater chance of developing dementia seven years later, despite the fact that there was no decline in cognitive functions in the LCP elderly group during the initial four years of follow-up (Shin et al. [33], in preparation). In view of these results, we propose that a cortical network consisting of bilateral precuneus, the right frontal gyrus, and the left middle frontal gyrus could be one of the key brain regions that constitute the neural substrates of reserve.

Correlation between precuneus volume and general cognition

An interesting finding of this study is that a measure of general cognition, the K-DRS Total score, correlated with the seed regions more strongly in the LCP compared to the NCP, especially in the precuneus. This is consistent with a previous study that found head circumference predicted the risk of dementia only in nuns with low but not high educational attainment; i.e., individuals with smaller head circumference had greater chance of developing dementia only in the low education group [51]. It appears that brain volume makes a greater difference in cognitive performance in individuals who lack or have low cognitive reserve, while it has limited effect in those with relatively high cognitive reserve. These findings of stronger association between brain size or volume and cognitive function are consistent with the cognitive reserve theory that proposes that individuals with higher reserve can cope with the pathology better via more efficient utilization of brain network [2], while those with low reserve will depend more on the size or the volume of the brain. More efficient utilization of the brain network assumed in the high cognitive reserve individuals could be mediated by stronger correlation or coherence of relevant cortical regions, as our findings suggest. Yet, the proposed cortical network was based on structural correlation of brain regions, which did not allow ascertaining the relationship between coherence and cognitive performance at an individual level. Studies investigating the functional connectivity from resting fMRI and its relation to cognitive function are needed to validate the proposed network.

precuneus and its associated network

While the prefrontal cortex has been acknowledged as the brain region involved in many higher-cognitive functions [51], the role of precuneus in various cognitive functions has been revealed only more recently, mainly through functional neuroimaging studies [52]. Nestled in the medial portion of the posterior parietal lobe (BA 7 and 31) with its rich connection to association cortices and subcortical structures, it is activated during a host of tasks that require various cognitive functions, such as retrieval of episodic memory [53], visuospatial imagery [54], working memory [55] reasoning [56] and self-relevant processing [57,58]. More recent connectivity studies have also found that the precuneus is one of the key hubs in the human brain where longdistance connections as well as local connections are abundant both structurally [59] and functionally [60], thereby playing a strategically important role in various cognitive functions. In particular, the central precuneus, where the gray matter volume difference was found between the two cognitive performance groups, has been demonstrated to be structurally and functionally connected to areas involving associative and higher-cognitive functions, such as the inferior parietal and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortices as well as the medial temporal area [61]. Therefore, the precuneus appears to be one of the optimal brain regions that could underlie overall cognitive functioning differences, and contribute to the neural substrate of reserve, which would waive off the impact of neuropathology in dementias. The fact that precuneus regions showed the greatest difference in the structural correlation map between the two cognitive performance groups further supports this hypothesis.

In terms of aging, the frontal lobe, specifically the prefrontal cortex, is the most vulnerable brain area [62,63], diminishing its volume at the rate of five percent every decade since adulthood in human. On the other hand, many studies have identified the precuneus as one of the brain regions that are affected early in AD [64,65] or even in its prodromal phase [66,67]. It is also a region that has lower brain glucose metabolism in the mild cognitive impairment elderly people who later converted to AD [68]. Smaller volumes and weaker connectivity from these two brain regions are likely to make the brain vulnerable to the effects of aging and degenerative changes, further supporting their role in moderating the development of dementia.

Caveats and Summary

Limitations of the study must be addressed. Although this study was part of a six-year follow-up study, this study in itself was crosssectional, so the inference about the risk of dementia based on the community-sample is indirect and should be interpreted with caution. We recognize that the sample size was very small, and comparison of groups in the correlation analyses did not reach the corrected threshold for multiple comparisons. Future studies, therefore, with larger sample with different population are necessary. However, we would like to note that since the definition of LCP is based on normative data, they make up only 5-15% of the community-residing elderly population.

In summary, this study has found areas in the brain that are likely to be part of neural networks supporting brain reserve. Bilateral precuneus and its associated network was found to be less developed in elderly people with low cognitive performance compared to the normal performing counterpart, and these regional differences could mediate the risk of dementia in elderly individuals. Elucidating the neural substrates of reserve that are associated with cognitive performance would have implications for developing neuro-cognitive tools or programs for elderly people who are at greater risk for dementia due to low cognitive reserve.

Acknowledgement

The authors would like to thank Kyungmin Shin, Hoyoung Kim, Myungjin Kim, Hojung Song, and Sungha Lee for their indispensable help in collecting neuropsychological data, Jiae Cheong for proofreading the manuscript, Jong-Won Kim for his genotype analysis, Jason Steffener for his invaluable comments on the manuscript.

This work was supported by a grant (#M103KV10021-03K2201-02130) from Brain Research Center of the 21st Century Frontier Research Program funded by the Ministry of Science and Technology of Republic of Korea, by the Brain Research Program (2015M3C7A1028376) through the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) funded by the Ministry of Science, ICT and Future Planning, and also by the ‘Overseas Training Expenses for Humanities and Social Sciences’ through Seoul National University (SNU) in 2014. The study abided by the Helsinki Accords [43], and obtained informed consent from all the volunteers, and paid an honorarium for their participation. The Seoul National Unviersity Institutional Review Board was founded after the study was conducted. No actual or potential conflicts of interest were present in this study.

References

--
Post your comment

Share This Article

Article Usage

  • Total views: 4104
  • [From(publication date):
    December-2016 - Jul 17, 2024]
  • Breakdown by view type
  • HTML page views : 3305
  • PDF downloads : 799
https://bahigox.fun/ https://bahisjet.fun/ https://bahsegel.fun/ https://betboo.fun/ https://betdoksan.xyz/ https://betebetgiris.xyz/ https://betexper.fun/ https://betgram.fun/ https://betkanyon.fun/ https://betkolik.fun/
Top