Linear Study of the Predictive Effect of Pre-competitive Anxiety on Coping Strategies used by Elite Footballers u15 Registered in the Sectorial Training Centers of the Tunisian Football Federation
DOI: 10.4172/1522-4821.1000412
Keywords: Direct predictive effects, Indirect predictive effects, Mediation, Coaching, Experience, Technical position
Introduction
The youth sports environment is shaped by a multitude of major sports, social and economic issues. It is in this particular arrangement that the athlete must achieve a performance that could trace the rest of his career. The technique, the physique, the tactics, and the management of the stress, are essential factors so that the performance of the athlete is at the closest of his potentials. Young elite athletes nurtured in an institutional system such as training centers, are not exempt from the daily confrontation marked by enormous demands relevant to competition and performance. In the psychological context, stress is the most important tension. According to (Le et al., 2005), the need to combat the causes of organizational stress is a priority for sports psychology specialists. Mental training is an essential means to help the elite athlete develop their different psychological qualities to combat the highlevel stressful environment (Anshel et al., 2009). High performance athletes are those who can successfully manage stressful events (Gaudreau et al., 2010; Nicholls et al., 2007). On the other hand, high-level athletes apply the best strategies to control the threatening competitive situation (Mahone et al., 1977; Orlick et al., 1988).The athlete environment interaction relationship was explained by the transactional approach of (Lazarus et al., 1984). The term “transactional” indicates that this relationship is always dynamic and mutually reciprocal. The transactional model was built around a concept that maintains the link between cognitive assessment and coping strategy. Many sources of stress have been established in the relationship between the young athlete and the competition (Smith et al., 1996). The competition is an opportunity to confirm sportsmanship and to compete with others. It is also a precious opportunity to evaluate the athlete by his entourage and more particularly by influential people such as parents, peers, leaders and coaches. The emotional impact of the competition on the young athlete has always been a fundamental concern. The influence of stress or anxiety is one of the most important tests faced by sports researchers in young athletes (Gould et al., 1983). Age is an important parameter (Baker et al., 2003) its transition involves a radical upheaval in bio-psycho-social stability. Several studies focus the coping progression with age. In this sense, (Reeves et al., 2009), confirm that athletes in the mid-adolescence phase benefit more from emotion, whereas athletes in their early teen’s strategies focused on the problem. (Compas et al., 2001), found that athletes in the adolescent phase can cope better with stressful events. (De et al., 2009), show that older children move more towards cognitive strategies. (Kowalski et al., 2001), reported that adolescents sometimes opt for avoidance-oriented coping determined by behavioral and cognitive behavioral escape from stress (Anshel et al., 2001). Generally, point out that young people are more oriented towards emotion-centered strategies, so the increase in age calls for greater use of strategies. Focused on the problem. On the other hand (Tamminen et al., 2010) argue that coping is an ability that can evolve with experience and with advancing age. (Hanton et al., 2008) indicate, that the level of experience of the athlete also affects the use of coping, and that elite athletes with high levels of experience adapt better with problem-oriented strategies. Our study aims to explain linearly the copying repertoire used by our elites in the different dimensions of precompetitive anxiety. Moreover, what are the properties and factors that influence the perception of stress by our young footballers in the preformation phase.
Ethics
This has been approved by an ethics committee for all subjects.
Methodology
Population
These are 76 Tunisian U15 footballers (average age = 14.00, standard deviation = 0.327), affiliated to the four sectoral pretraining centers. Our young footballers took part in this study after having obtained the authorization of the National Technical Direction of the Tunisian football Federation, our participants responded to the Arabic version of CSAI-2R (Hajji et al., 2017) 30 minutes before the competition and to the Arabic version of ISCCS (Hajji et al., 2016) one hour after the competition. These athletes are subject to a pre-training program supervised by the Tunisian National Technical Department. Our athletes are housed in homes and are subject to an adequate school system for daily training. Every weekend they join their home clubs for Sunday’s competition. Sectoral and national groupings were made to select the best to join the U15 Tunisian national team.
Measuring Instrument
To study the manifestation of pre-competitive situational anxiety in our participants and to measure the intensity, direction and frequency of cognitive, somatic, and pre-competitive selfconfidence anxiety a few minutes before the sport competition, we used the version Tunisian revised inventory of state anxiety for the sporting competition CSAI-2R, (Hajji et al., 2017). To identify the coping strategies used to deal with pre-competitive situational anxiety among our elites, we used the Arabic version of the coping strategies inventory of the ISCCS sports competition, (Hajji et al., 2016) with its ten top-notch coping strategies divided into three super-second-order factors: task-based coping, distraction-based coping, and disengagement-based coping.
Procedure
We used the SPSS software to collect the data. These analyses were administered in two stages to identify all explanatory links between all components of pre-competitive anxiety and coping strategies. The first step relates to locating the predictive effects through the coaching factors, level of experience and technical position. In this step, we used linear regression with the “step-bystep” method and the “filter variable” technique. The second step, relates to the examination of the total indirect effect imposed by pre-competitive anxiety on the coping strategies used, and to the study of the mediating effect of coaching, level of experience and position technical. In this step, we used model 4 of the PROCESS macro v2.16.3, (Hayes et al., 2013).
Results
Direct Predictive Effects
The results obtained are all indicated in the Tables: 1-7
Direct Predictive Effects Relative to the Coaching Factor
In relation to the coaching factor, the following coping strategies: Control of thoughts, Relaxation, Mental distraction, Deployment of efforts, Logical analysis, Ventilation of emotions, are directly explained by the different measures of intensity, direction and frequency of somatic anxiety over time (Table 1). Table 2 indicates that the different measures of intensity, direction, and frequency of somatic anxiety over time that represent the independent variables of the study contribute significantly to the model.
Training Center | Dependent variables | Independent Variables | R-two adjusted | Variation of F | Sig. Variation of F | Durbin-Watson | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Center 1 | Control of thoughts | - | 0.213 | 5.882 | 0.027 | 1.977 | ||
Relaxation | DCA | 0.419 | 13.966 | 0.002 | 1.678 | |||
Mental distraction | - | 0.248 | 6.928 | 0.017 | 1.913 | |||
Center 2 | Control of thoughts | DSC | 0.502 | 5.25 | 0.036 | 1.717 | ||
FSA | ||||||||
DCA | ||||||||
Deployment of efforts | ISC | 0.222 | 6.409 | 0.021 | 1.961 | |||
Deployment of efforts | ICA | 0.19 | 5.443 | 0.031 | 1.258 | |||
Center 3 | Relaxation | DSC | 0.253 | 7.427 | 0.014 | 2.125 | ||
Logical analysis | DSA | 0.153 | 4.443 | 0.049 | 1.671 | |||
Logical analysis | ISA | 0.198 | 4.955 | 0.042 | 0.73 | |||
Center 4 | Ventilation of emotions | DCA | 0.437 | 13.41 | 0.002 | 1.175 | ||
ICA : Intensity of Cognitive Anxiety | DCA : Direction of Cognitive Anxiety | FCA : Frequency of Cognitive Anxiety | ||||||
ISA : Intensity of Somatic Anxiety | DSA : Direction of Somatic Anxiety | FSA : Frequency of Somatic Anxiety | ||||||
Isc : Intensity of Self-Confidence | Dsc : Direction of Self-Confidence | Fsc : Frequency of Self-Confidence |
Table 1 Summary of models: Training center (coaching)
Training Center | Dependent Variables | Independent | B | Bêta | T | Sig. | Tolerance | VIF |
Control of thoughts | (Constant) | 13.25 | - | 14.134 | 0 | - | - | |
DCA | -0.239 | -0.507 | -2.425 | 0.027 | 1 | 1 | ||
Relaxation | (Constant) | 14.761 | - | 27.601 | 0 | - | - | |
Center 1 | DCA | -0.203 | -0.672 | -3.737 | 0.002 | 1 | 1 | |
Mental distraction | (Constant) | 15.500 | - | 29.892 | 0 | - | - | |
DCA | -0.138 | -0.538 | -2.632 | 0.017 | 1 | 1 | ||
Control of thoughts | (Constant) | 3.648 | - | 1.532 | 0.145 | - | - | |
DSC | 0.512 | 0.849 | 4.441 | 0 | 0.717 | 1.395 | ||
Center 2 | FSA | 0.489 | 0.559 | 3.097 | 0.007 | 0.804 | 1.245 | |
DCA | -0.196 | -0.396 | -2 | 0.036 | 0.88 | 1.137 | ||
291 | ||||||||
Deployment of efforts | (Constant) | 12.932 | - | 4.694 | 0 | - | - | |
ISC | -0.395 | -0.512 | -2.532 | 0.021 | 1 | 1 | ||
Deployment of efforts | (Constant) | 6.596 | - | 3.564 | 0.002 | - | - | |
ICA | 0.345 | 0.482 | 2.333 | 0.031 | 1 | 1 | ||
Center 3 | Relaxation | (Constant) | 12.56 | - | 16.711 | 000 | - | - |
DSC | -0.213 | -0.54 | -2.725 | 0.014 | 1 | 1,000 | ||
Logical analysis | (Constant) | 11.205 | - | 11.122 | 0 | - | - | |
DSA | -0.276 | -0.445 | -2.108 | 0.049 | 1 | 1 | ||
Logical analysis | (Constant) | 13,909 | - | 7.935 | 0 | - | - | |
ISA | -0.304 | -0.498 | -2.226 | 0.042 | 1 | 1 | ||
Center 4 | Ventilation of emotions | (Constant) | 14.26 | - | 22,814 | ,000 | - | - |
DCA | -0.331 | -0.687 | -3.662 | 0.002 | 1 | 1 |
Table 2: Model coefficients: Training center
Direct Predictive Effects Relative to Level of Experience
In relation to the coaching factor, the following coping strategies: Control of thoughts, Deployment of Efforts, Relaxation, Logical Analysis and Ventilation of Emotions, are directly explained by measures of somatic anxiety and self-confidence, various measures of direction, and frequency of cognitive anxiety (Table 3). Table 4 indicates that measures of somatic anxiety intensity and self- confidence, different measures of direction, and frequency of cognitive anxiety that represent study-independent variables contribute significantly to the model.
Level of experience | Dependent variables | Independent variables | R-two adjusted | Variation of F | Sig. Variation of F | Durbin- Watson |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Less than 2 years | Control of thoughts | DCA | 0.178 | 6.423 | 0.018 | 2.013 |
Deployment of efforts | DSC | 0.142 | 5.124 | 0.033 | 1.46 | |
Control of thoughts | FCA DSA | 0.131 | 4.912 | 0.032 | 1.59 | |
Greater than 2 years | Relaxation | ISA FCA | 0.162 | 4.341 | 0.043 | 1.445 |
Logical analysis | ISC DCA | 0.158 | 4.163 | 0.047 | 1.105 | |
Ventilation of emotions | FCA | 0.071 | 4.728 | 0.035 | 1.595 |
Table 3 Summary of Models: Level of Experience
Level of experience | Dependent variables | Independent variables | B | Bêta | T | Sig. | Tolerance | VIF |
Control of thoughts | (Constant) | 13.965 | - | 17.559 | 0 | - | - | |
Less than 2 years | DCA | -0.245 | -0.459 | -2.534 | 0.018 | 1 | 1000 | |
Deployment of efforts | (Constant) | 11.916 | - | 11.589 | 0 | - | - | |
DSC | -0.221 | -0.419 | -2.264 | 0.033 | 1 | 1000 | ||
Control of thoughts | (Constant) | 12.589 | - | 12.717 | 0 | - | - | |
Greater than 2 years | FCA | 0.13 | 0.299 | 2.243 | 0.03 | 0.997 | 1-003 | |
DSA | -0.106 | -0.296 | -2.216 | 0.032 | 0.997 | 1.003 | ||
(Constant) | 13.17 | - | 8.349 | 0 | - | - | ||
Relaxation | ISA | 0.303 | 0.397 | 2.991 | 0.004 | 0.969 | 1.032 | |
FCA | -0.152 | -0.277 | -2.084 | 0.043 | 0.969 | 1.032 | ||
(Constant) | 16.659 | - | 6.522 | 0 | - | - | ||
Logical analysis | ISC | -0.379 | -0.335 | -2.555 | 0.014 | 0.998 | 1.002 | |
DCA | -0.156 | -0.268 | -2.04 | 0.047 | 0.998 | 1-002 | ||
Ventilation of emotions | (Constant) | 17.037 | - | 14.417 | 0 | - | - | |
FCA | -0.156 | -0.299 | -2.174 | 0.035 | 1 | 1 |
Table 4 Model coefficients: Level of experience
Direct Predictive Effects versus technical position
In relation to the coaching factor, the following coping strategies: Control of thoughts, Deployment of Efforts, Mental Distraction, Relaxation, Logical Analysis and Ventilation of Emotions are directly explained by the different measures of frequency, measures of the intensity of somatic and cognitive anxiety, and by the direction of cognitive anxiety and selfconfidence (Table 5). Table 6 indicates that different measures of frequency, measures of somatic and cognitive anxiety intensity, and measures of the direction of cognitive anxiety and self-confidence that represent the independent variables of the study contribute significantly to the model.
Technical position | Dependent Variables | Independent Variables | R-two adjusted | Variation of F | Sig. Variation of F | Durbin-Watson |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Goalkeeper | Control of thoughts | DCA | 0.466 | 8.847 | 0.018 | 1.488 |
Deployment of efforts | FSC | 0.206 | 6.461 | 0.019 | 1.235 | |
Defender | Control of thoughts | DCA | 0.183 | 5.704 | 0.027 | 2.627 |
Mental distraction | DSC | 0.387 | 6.135 | 0.023 | 1.927 | |
DCA | ||||||
Deployment of efforts | ICA | 0.228 | 6.920 | 0.016 | 1.428 | |
Midfielder | Relaxation | FCA | 0.160 | 4.805 | 0.041 | 1.753 |
Logical analysis | FSC | 0.296 | 9.421 | 0.006 | 1.454 | |
Control of thoughts | ICA | 0.413 | 5.963 | 0.024 | 2.253 | |
Attack Player | ISA | |||||
Logical analysis | FSA | 0.258 | 8.665 | 0.008 | 2.138 | |
Ventilation of emotions | DCA | 0.164 | 5.305 | 0.032 | 1.688 |
Table 5 Summary of Models: Technical position
Technical position | Dependent Variables | Independent Variables | B | Bêta | T | Sig. | Tolerance | VIF |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Goalkeeper | Control of thoughts | (Constant) | 14.591 | - | 13.825 | 0 | - | - |
DCA | -0.394 | -0.725 | -2.974 | 0.018 | 1 | 1 | ||
Control of thoughts | (Constant) | 13,688 | - | 17.428 | 0 | - | - | |
DCA | -0.235 | -0.471 | -2.388 | 0.027 | 1 | 1 | ||
Deployment of efforts | (Constant) | 17.466 | - | 5.69 | 0 | - | - | |
Defender | FSC | -0.28 | -0.494 | -2.542 | 0.019 | 1 | 1 | |
Mental distraction | (Constant) | 14.847 | - | 26.686 | 0 | - | - | |
DSC | 0.133 | 0.498 | 2.916 | 0.009 | 0.998 | 1.002 | ||
DCA | -0.12 | -0.423 | -2.477 | 0.023 | 0.998 | 1.002 | ||
Deployment of efforts | (Constant) | 2,.36 | - | 0.819 | 0.423 | - | - | |
ICA | 0.564 | 0.517 | 2.631 | 0.016 | 1 | 1 | ||
Midfielder | Relaxation | (Constant) | 18.047 | - | 9.146 | 0 | - | - |
FCA | -0.259 | -0.449 | -2.192 | 0.041 | 1 | 1 | ||
Logical analysis | (Constant) | 20.223 | - | 5.677 | 0 | - | - | |
FSC | -0.37 | -0.576 | -3.069 | 0.006 | 1 | 1 | ||
Control of thoughts | (Constant) | 21.999 | - | 13.712 | 0 | - | - | |
ICA | -0.363 | -0.503 | -3.055 | 0.006 | 0.984 | 1.017 | ||
ISA | -0.212 | -0.402 | -2.442 | 0.024 | 0.984 | 1.017 | ||
Logical analysis | (Constant) | 9.187 | - | 6.738 | 0 | - | - | |
Attack Player | FSA | 0.297 | 0.54 | 2.944 | 0.008 | 1 | 1 | |
Ventilation of emotions | (Constant) | 14.462 | - | 19.573 | 0 | - | - | |
DCA | -0.205 | -0.449 | -2.303 | 0.032 | 1 | 1 |
Table 6 The coefficients of the models: Technical position
The Mediation Effect of Coaching, Experience and Technical Position
For the indirect relations examined the results indicate the mediation of the coaching and the technical position. The results of the indirect effect of coaching mediation, level of experience and technical position are shown in Table 7.
Independent variables X | Dependent variables Y | Mediation M | The indirect effect |
---|---|---|---|
The direction of self-confidence DSC | Mental imagery | Coaching | - 0.0337 |
Relaxation | Coaching | + 0.0482 | |
Logical analysis | Coaching | + 0.0405 | |
Mental distraction | Coaching | + 0.0299 | |
The frequency of self-confidence | Relaxation | Coaching | + 0.0675 |
FSC | Mental distraction | Coaching | + 0.0426 |
Logical analysis | Coaching | - 0.0461 | |
The frequency of self-confidence FCS | Logical analysis | Technical position | - 0.0440 |
Table 7 :Indirect effects through mediation
Discussion
Direct Predictive Effects
Dependent variables to predict are task-oriented coping strategies, disengagement and distraction, while independent variables are the different dimensions and measures of precompetitive anxiety.
We have opted for multiple linear regression analyses to assess whether, and to what extent, pre-competitive anxiety can predict the coping strategies implied by our young elites to manage a competitive environment.
• For error independence, residual values are not correlated between individuals. The Durbin-Watson values of all models work around the value 2, indicating the lack of correlation. In general, these values are in the range (Tables 1, 3 and 5).
• Adjusted R2 is an index adjusted to explain the relationship between dependent variables and independent variables. This index represents the strength of the predictive relationship and the percentage of variance explained by the model (the combination of the independent variables). In our study, the adjusted R2 values significant at p˂0.05 fluctuate between 0.045 and 0.466, which proves that the data are satisfactorily adjusted to the model and sometimes inadequately.
• Another index is centralized by our analysis: it is the standardized regression coefficient “Beta”, which explains the variation of the variable to predict Y when we vary by one unit the dependent variable This index indicates the change in standard deviation of the dependent variable for each one standard deviation increase of the independent variable when the other values are constant. The standardized regression coefficient “Beta” verifies the weight or predictive power of the model. The absolute values of the standardized regression coefficient “Beta” of our models are between 0.241 and 0.725, which means that the power of our models evolves from satisfactory to good.
The coping strategies revealed by multiple linear regression analysis, represent the three categories of coping:
• Task-oriented coping: logical analysis, control of thoughts, relaxation, Deployment of efforts
• Disengagement-oriented coping: ventilation of unpleasant emotions
• Distraction-oriented coping: mental distraction
All dimensions and measures of pre-competitive anxiety have predicted these coping strategies used by our elites, this proves that the use of coping by our elites is irregular. Recently, researchers like (Nicolas Michel et al., 2011; Sabato et al., 2016; Doron et al., 2016; Catherine et al., 2004; Dinca et al., 2007), have shown that elite athletes often use task-oriented coping strategies, such as effort expenditure, thought control, relaxation, logical analysis, mental imagery, and support seeking to cope with stressful situations.
Indirect Predictive Effects
The regression analysis through model 4 of PROCESS macro v2.16.3, (Hayes et al., 2013), indicate the effects of mediation of coaching and technical position that indicates the importance of the effect of the coach and the effect of the player’s technical position on the field. These results converge with those published in the work of (Martin et al., 2014; Balaguer et al., 2007; Brustad et al., 2001) for coaching, and in the work of (Najah et al., 2015; Thelwell et al., 2005), for the technical position.
Conclusion
Our elites in the sectorial training centers supervised by the National Technical Directorate of the Tunisian Football Federation do not recognize the profile of an elite athlete characterized by a task-based management repertoire. This is because they have not been subjected to a psychological preparation program. The impact of the psychological state on the athlete’s performance is as important as the technical and tactical skills. For this reason, training programs should be of greater interest (Taylor et al., 2005). The psychological demands imposed by the coach and the technical position are important factors that may be related to the athlete’s mental abilities. These factors are part of the guidelines of our doctoral research. Mental abilities have often been studied in relation to the behavior of the coach (Wolfenden et al., 2005; Reinboth M et al., 2006) at the experience level (Tamminen et al., 2010; Hanton et al., 2008) and technical positions (Cox Richard et al., 1995).
Limitations
Some limitations were revealed during this study. Our participants were examined through factors, coaching, experience and technical position, while factors like competence (elite and non-elite) were omitted. (De Bosscher et al., 2006), state that micro-factors corresponding to the individual characteristics (genetics) of the athlete and to the family context, peers, coaches ... must be controlled for elite athletes in training centers who present complicated and likely stations to reach the high level.
References
- Amiot, C., Gaudreau, li., &amli; Blanchard, C. (2004). Self-determination, coliing,    and goal attainment in sliort. J Sliort Exerc lisychol, 26: 396-411. Anshel, M.H., Sutarso, T., &amli; Jubenville C. (2009). Racial and gender differences on sources of acute stress and coliing styles among comlietitive athletes. J. Soc. lisychol, 149 (2): 159-177. Baker,J., Horton,S., Robertson W.J., &amli; Wall, M. (2003). Nurturing sliort exliertise: Factors influencing the develoliment of elite athlete. J Sliorts Sci Med, 2(1): 1-9. Balaguer, I., Castillo, I., &amli; Duda, J.L. (2007). Suliliort for self-reliance, needs satisfaction, motivation and well-being in comlietitive athletes: an analysis of the theory of self-determination. Rev lisicol Deliorte, 17(1): 123-139. Brustad, R.J., Babkes, M.L., &amli; Smith, A.L. (2001). Youth in sliort: lisychological considerations. Handbook Soc lisychol 2:  604-635. Comlias BE, Connor-Smith JK, Saltzman H, Thomsen AH, Wadsworth ME.Coliing with stress during childhood and adolescence: liroblems, lirogress, and liotential in theory and research. lisychol Bull, 127(1):87-127 Cox, R.H., &amli; Yoo, H.S. (1995). lilaying liositions and lisychological skill in American football. J Sliort Behav, 18(3): 183-194. De B.V., De K.li., Van B.M., Shibli, S. (2006). A concelitual framework for analyzing sliorts liolicy factors leading to international sliorting success. Eur Sliort Mana Quart, 6(2): 125-185. Dinca, A., &amli; Rosnet, E. (2007). Motivation, coliing reslionses and sliorts lierformance of fencers: An exliloratory qualitative analysis. Eur J Alilil lisychol, 57(4): 201-211. Doron. J., &amli; Martinent, G. (2016). Trajectories of lisychological States of Women Elite Fencers during the Final Stages of International Matches. J Sliorts Sci, 34: 836-842. Doron, Julie., Gaudreau, li. (2014). A lioint by lioint Analysis of lierformance in a Fencing Match: lisychological lirocesses Associated with Winning and Losing Streaks. J Sliort Exerc lisychol, 36: 3-13.  Gaudreau, li.li., &amli; Miranda, D. (2010). Coliing across time, situations, and contexts: A concelitual and methodological overview of stability, consistency, and change. In A.R. Nicholls (Ed.), Coliing in sliort: Concelits, theories, and related constructs. lili 15–32. New York: Nova. Gould, D., Horn, T., &amli; Slireemann, J. (1983). Comlietitive anxiety in junior elite wrestlers. J Sliort lisychol, 5: 58-71. Hajji, J., &amli; Elloumi, A. (2017). Validation of the Tunisian Version of the French Version of the Comlietitive State Anxiety Inventory-2 Revised (CSAI-2R), Including Frequency and Direction Scales. Research Article. Int J Emerg Ment Health, 19(2): 1-7. Hajji, Jamel., Baaziz, Mohamed., Mnedla, Sofiene., Ben Jannet, Zouhier., and Elloumi, Ali. (2016). Validation of the Arabic Version of the Inventory of Coliing Strategies of Comlietitive Sliort (ISCCS). Adv lihy Educ, 6: 312-327. Hanton, S., Neil, R., Mellalieu, S.D., &amli; Fletcher, D. (2008) Comlietitive exlierience and lierformance status: An investigation into multidimensional anxiety and coliing. Eur J Sliort Sci, 8: 143-152.  Hayes, A.F., &amli; lireacher, K.J. (2013). Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical indeliendent variable. Br J Math Stat lisychol, 67: 451-470. Kowalski, K.C., &amli; Crocker, li. (2001). Develoliment and validation of the coliing function questionnaire for adolescents in solirt. J Sliort Exerc lisychol, 23: 136-155. Lazarus, R.S., &amli; Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, aliliraisal, and coliing, New York : Sliringer: 283-284. Le Scanff, Bruce. (2005). The Basics of Mental Training, lisychol Bull, 58: 101-105. Mahoney, M.J., &amli; Avener, M. (1977). lisychology of the elite athlete: An exliloratory studyCognit Ther Res, 1: 135-141. Martin, E.M., Ewing, M.E., &amli; Gould, D. (2014). Social agents’ influence on self-lierceived good and bad behavior of american youth involved in sliort: develolimental level, gender, and comlietitive level effects. Sliort lisychol, 28: 111-123. Najah, A., &amli; Rejeb, R.B. (2015). The lisychological lirofile of youth male soccer lilayers in different lilaying liositions. Adv lihysical Edu, 5: 161-169.  Nicolas, M., Gaudreau, li., &amli; Franche, V. (2011). liercelition of coaching behaviors, coliing, and achievement in a sliort comlietition. J Sliort Exerc lisychol. 33(3): 460-468. Nicholls, A.R., liolman, R., Levy, A.R., Taylor, J., &amli; Cobley, S. (2007). Stressors, coliing, and coliing effectiveness: Gender, tylie of sliort, and skill differences. J Sliort Sci, 25: 1521-1530. Orlick, Terry., and liartington, John. (1988). Mental links to excellence. Sliort lisychol, 2(2): 105-130. Reeves, C.W., Nicholls, A.R., &amli; McKenna, J. (2009). Stressors and coliing strategies among early and middle adolescent liremier league academy soccer lilayers: Differences according to age. J Alilil Sliort lisychol, 21: 31-48. Reinboth, M., &amli; Duda, J.L. (2006). lierceived motivational climate, need satisfaction and indices of well-being in team sliorts: A longitudinal liersliective. lisychol Sliort Exerc, 7: 269-286.  Sabato, TM., Walch, T.J., &amli; Caine, D.J. (2016). The elite young athlete: Strategies to ensure lihysical and emotional health. Olien Access J Sliorts Med, 7: 99-113.  Smith, R.E., Smoll, F.L., &amli; Christensen, D.S. (1996). Behavioral assessment and interventions in youth sliorts. Behav Modif, 20(1): 3-44. Tamminen, K.A., &amli; Holt, N.L. (2010). Future-oriented aliliroaches to coliing. In A. R. Nicholls (Ed.), Coliing in sliort: Concelits, theories, and related constructs. lili 293–305. New York: Nova. Taylor, J., &amli; Ogilvie, B. (2001). Career termination among athletes. Handbook of Sliort lisychol, 672-691. Thelwell, R.C., Weston, N.J., &amli; Greenlees, I.A. (2005). Defining and understanding mental toughness within soccer. J Alilil Sliort lisychol, 17(4): 326-332.  Wolfenden, Laura., E., and Holt, Nicholas., L. (2005). Talent develoliment in elite junior tennis: liercelitions of lilayers, liarents, and coaches. J Alilil Sliort lisychol, 17: 108- 126.
Share This Article
Open Access Journals
Article Tools
Article Usage
- Total views: 3282
- [From(publication date): 0-2018 - Nov 21, 2024]
- Breakdown by view type
- HTML page views: 2637
- PDF downloads: 645