ISSN: 2472-5005

Journal of Speech Pathology & Therapy
Open Access

Our Group organises 3000+ Global Conferenceseries Events every year across USA, Europe & Asia with support from 1000 more scientific Societies and Publishes 700+ Open Access Journals which contains over 50000 eminent personalities, reputed scientists as editorial board members.

Open Access Journals gaining more Readers and Citations
700 Journals and 15,000,000 Readers Each Journal is getting 25,000+ Readers

This Readership is 10 times more when compared to other Subscription Journals (Source: Google Analytics)

Evaluation of Home Communication Skills in Children with Speech Delay

Seda Keten1, Ayse Sanem Sahli2* and Mesut Kaya3
1Vocational School of Health Services, Istanbul Gelisim University, Istanbul, Turkey
2Hearing and Speech Training Center, Vocational School of Health Services, Hacettepe University, Ankara, Turkey
3Department of ENT, Special Ilgi Ear Hospital, Ankara, Turkey
*Corresponding Author: Ayse Sanem Sahli, Vocational School of Health Services, Hearing and Speech Training Center, Hacettepe University, Ankara, Turkey, Tel: +90-535-659-4775, Email: ssahli@hacettepe.edu.tr

Received: 04-Jun-2018 / Accepted Date: 26-Sep-2018 / Published Date: 01-Oct-2018 DOI: 10.4172/2472-5005.1000137

Abstract

Aim: The purpose of this study is to examine the communication skills of children with speech delay and to examine the impact of the child's gender, age, family type, attendance at kindergarten, socio-economic level of the family, parental occupation, educational status on the child’s communication skills.
Materials and methods: Parents of 100 children between 3 and 6 yrs of age who were diagnosed with delayed speech were included. Parents' General Information Form and Home Communication Questionnaire were applied through face-to-face interview technique. Turkish Preschool Language Scale (TPLS)-5 was used in evaluating children's language and speech skills.
Results: 66% of children with delayed speech who participated in this study are boys and 34% are girls. When children spend time with adults, 89% of them play physical games and 83% learning games, 79% check books and 67% do daily work. The time children spend at home is as follows; 13.76 h with the mother, 4.76 h with the siblings, 4.51 h with the father, 1.24 h with the other family members, and 0.19 h with the other persons. Children spend an average of 2.79 h watching TV at the weekend, and 2.34 h during the weekdays on a daily basis.
Conclusion: The increasing use of erroneous and long-lasting technology (computer, tablet, etc.) in recent years constructs the basis for speech delay in early childhood. Especially important is the effective use of speech between the ages 0 and 3, the most critical period for language and speech development, by increasing educational verbal communication in the home environment and by keeping children away from intense visual stimuli as much as possible.

Keywords: Speech; Speech delay; Children; Home; Communication

Introduction

One of the most important means of communication among people is speech. It is a narration-focused language skill. The narration is necessary for the speech to take place; the ability to understand and symbolize, the language, the grammar of the native language, knowledge and vocabulary are other essential processes. These processes are realized in the mind of the individual, which results with the construction of the meaning and verbal expression [1-3].

Language is a code system with rules that communities share in order to communicate. When the individual learns the code system of a language, he/she will learn the language. Language development is examined under the headings of recipient and expressive languages. The recipient language is the ability of an individual to understand and interpret the language-specific codes. Expressive language is the language skill that is used to convey the individual's thoughts, feelings and desires to other individuals. Tools for expressive language are eye contact, sign, gesture, mimic and speech [3,4]. The language develops as an interaction of individual since he/she was born with environmental elements parallel to the sensory and cognitive development [4]. Due to the development and maturity of brain in the process up to the age of three, language and speech development progresses very rapidly. The complete acquisition of speech takes place as a result of a process and requires all rules of the language to be learned [5]. Delayed language and speech appears as the mismatch of the chronological and speech ages in the development process. The child has a speech development which obviously differs from the speech of his peers. The child will be considered as having a delay in language and speech if his/her way of speaking is different from his/her peers in a way that other individuals cannot understand. As a result of this situation, which is frequent during childhood, the professional life and social skills of the child are adversely affected. One of the most important reasons for the delay in the development of speech in children is the characteristics of the home communication environment [1-6].

The main purpose of our study is to examine the home communication skills of children with speech delay. In addition, the effect of the variables such as gender, age, family type, kindergarten attendance, socio-economic level, parents' occupation, educational status, television watching time, computer/tablet usage duration on communication skills is examined.

Materials and Methods

This study was initiated after the decision of the Faculty of Medicine, Clinical Researches Ethics Committee dated 16.03.2016 dated 2015/12 and numbered 99950669/263 which stated that the study is in compliance with ethical and scientific principles. The sample of the study consisted of parents (mother and father) of 100 children between 3 and 6 yrs of age who were diagnosed with language and speech delay according to the Preschool Language Scale-5 (PLS-5) test conducted in Hacettepe University Hearing and Speech Training Center. Otologic examinations and audiologic assessments were performed for all the children. Parents of children with an additional disability and/or illness other than delayed speech (hearing loss, mental retardation, neurological diseases, etc.) are excluded from the study. The Home Communication Questionnaire and the General Information Form were applied by the researchers through face-to-face interview technique to the parents of 100 children who experienced a speech delay.

To get parents’ permission, they were asked to sign the Information Consent Form and Voluntary Participation Form giving information about the study.

Data collection tools

General information form: The 'General Information Form' consists of questions for the child and the parents. The form is used to determine the factors that may affect the speech development of the children participating in the study such as chronological age, gender, parental education status and occupation, number of siblings, age of toilet training acquisition, age of suspected speech delay, birth order and the educational institution student attended at pre-school level.

Preschool Language Scale-5 (PLS-5) and home communication questionnaire

PLS-5 is a language assessment test widely used throughout the world to identify children with Auditory Perception (AP) and Expressive Communication (EC) delay or impairment, developed for use in children aged 0-7 yrs and 11 months [7]. PLS-5 was translated, adapted, and reliability and validity studies were conducted by Sahli and Belgin on a total of 1320 children between 0 and 7 yrs and 11 months of age. The Pearson correlation coefficients for the standard scales of Turkish PLS-5 resulted as follows in the end of the study- AP Raw Score:0.937, EC Raw Score:0.908 and TLS (Total Language Score) is 0.926. The correlation coefficients for age equivalence are, AP:0.871, EC:0.896 and TLS:0.887. The study results indicate that TPLS-5 is a valid and reliable language test for Turkish children [8].

PLS-5 consists of two standard scales, Auditory Perception and Expressive Communication; and three additional measurements, Language Sample Test, Articulation Screening Test, and Home Communication Questionnaire [7,8]. The Home Communication Questionnaire was prepared for children between 0 and 2 yrs and 11 months of age. It is a questionnaire to determine the communication behaviors of the person(s) taking care of the child with the child. It can also be used to assess communication skills of older children. Questionnaire includes questions about how the child's receptive and expressive language skills are in different settings. The Home Communication Questionnaire can also be used to support the results of the PLS-5 test and to prepare a service plan for families [7,8].

Statistical analysis

The data obtained as a result of the study were analyzed with the SPSS 20 statistical program. The frequency, percentage, minimum and maximum values, mean, standard deviation, Chi-square (χ2) and eta coefficients were calculated. The significance level of the interpretation of the results was accepted as 0.05.

Results

66% of the children (N:66) participated in our study were boys and 34% (N:34) were girls. When the age range of children is examined; 35% are between the ages of 3.0-4.6 yrs, and 65% are between the ages of 4.6-6.0 yrs. 91% of the families have the nuclear family type, whereas 9% have the extended family type. 76% of the children had pre-school education and 24% did not receive pre-school education (Table 1).

    N %
Gender of child Boy 66 66,0
Girl 34 34,0
Child's age 3:0 – 4:6 years 35 35.0
4:6- 6:0 years 65 65.0
Family type Nuclear Family 91 91.0
Extended family 9 9.0
Status of kindergarten attendance Attended 76 76.0
Not attended 24 24.0

Table 1: Demographic information of children participated in study.

33% of the parents who participated in the research are high school graduates. 73% of the mothers are housewives and 61% of the fathers are employed in private sector. 48% of the families have a medium socio-economic level (Table 2).

    N %
Mother's education level Primary School 26 26.0
Secondary School 11 11.0
High School 33 33.0
Associate Degree 6 6.0
Undergraduate 22 22.0
Graduate 2 2.0
Father's education level Primary School 15 15.0
Secondary School 15 15.0
High School 33 33.0
Associate Degree 5 5.0
Undergraduate 26 26.0
Graduate 6 6.0
Mother's occupation Housewife 73 73.0
Public Employee 20 20.0
Private Sector Employee 7 7.0
Self-Employed 0 0.0
Other 0 0.0
Father's occupation Housewife 2 2.0
Public Employee 31 31.0
Private Sector Employee 61 61.0
Self-Employed 6 6.0
Other 0 0.0
Socio-economic level Low 19 19.0
Average 48 48.0
Good 29 29.0
Very Good 4 4.0

Table 2: Demographic information of parents who have child with speech delay.

The average time children spend at home is 13.76 h with mother, 4.51 h with father, 4.76 h with siblings, 1.24 h with other family members and 0.19 h with other people. The children who participated in our study watch television for 2.79 h at weekends and 2.34 h on weekdays; play with other children 2.05 h at weekends, 2.30 h on weekdays; play with adults 1.88 h at weekends and 1.51 h on weekdays; play games on their own 1.69 h at weekends and 1.55 h on weekdays; and use computer or tablet for 0.99 h (Table 3).

  My child is usually at home…   N   Min   Max.   Mean   SD
Tv monitoring –weekdays (hour) 100 1 10 2.34 1.38
Tv monitoring –weekend (hour) 100 0 10 2.79 1.72
Playing with adults-weekdays(hour) 100 0 12 1.51 1.76
Playing with adults-weekend(hour) 100 0 12 1.88 1.90
Playing with other children-weekdays (hour) 100 0 8 2.30 1.66
Playing with other children-weekend (hour) 100 0 6 2.05 1.41
Self-playing -weekdays(hour) 100 0 6 1.55 1.23
Self-playing -weekend(hour) 100 0 7 1.69 1.48
Tablet-computer usage (hour) 100 0 4 0.94 0.86

Table 3: Activities and durations of the child in the home environment. Min: Minimum; Max: Maximum; SD: Standard Deviation.

Favorite TV programs of 74% of children are cartoons, 17% of children are children's programs and 9% of children are movies. Children spend 89% of their time with adults playing physical games, 83% learning, 79% reading books and 67% doing daily work (Table 4).

When my child spends time with adults... Y/N  N %
Conducts daily work Yes 67 67.0
No 33 33.0
Plays physical games Yes 89 89.0
No 11 11.0
Plays games intended to learn Yes 83 83.0
No 17 17.0
Looks at books Yes 79 79.0
No 21 21.0

Table 4: Activities conducted by child while spending time with adults.

Children who participated in the research play games with other children on weekdays and at weekends; 60% share their toys with and talk to other children; 58% follow the instructions of other children or do what they say when playing on weekdays and at weekends (Table 5). Among the children who participated in the research, 94% play well with their toys, 89% play with two or more toys at the same time, 81% play simple games with parents, 80% realize known situations, 72% use their toys for symbolic games, 71% play by shaking and hitting objects one another, and 53% play unknown situations.

When playing with other children, my child sometimes...   N %
Plays with other children but does not share his/her toys No 56 56.0
Weekday 10 10.0
Weekend 3 3.0
Weekdays and Weekend 31 31.0
Plays with other children and does what they do or what they say No 35 35.0
Weekday 6 6.0
Weekend 1 1.0
Weekday and Weekend 58 58.0
Plays with other children, tells them what they should do No 59 59.0
Weekday 5 5.0
Weekend 2 2.0
Weekday and weekend 34 34.0
Plays with other children by sharing his/her toys and speaking with them No 34 34.0
Weekday 5 5.0
Weekend 1 1.0
Weekday and weekend 60 60.0
My child does not play with other children No 97 97.0
Yes 3 3.0

Table 5: Activities Conducted By Child While Playing with Peers.

93% of the children responded to the voices, 93% raised their head and stopped what they were engaged in when their name was called, 90% looked at the direction indicated by the "look" direction, 89% looked at the talking person, 80% looked at the food stool or food falling from the table. 7% of the children do not react and they do not raise their head and stop what they were engaged in when their name was called (Table 6).

My child is attentive to things happening around him/her. For instance, Y/N N %
Reacts to voices Yes 93 93.0
No 7 7.0
When I call his/her name, he/she lifts head by stopping what he/she is engaged at Yes 93 93.0
No 7 7.0
Looks at talking people Yes 89 89.0
No 11 11.0
Looks at food falling down from food stool/table Yes 80 80.0
No 20 20.0
When I say "Look", he/she stares at the place I point Yes 90 90.0
No 10 10.0

Table 6: Attention of child to exterior stimuli.

When parents of delayed children take care of them, 96% are smiling, 89% are talking when parents speak, 89% are inviting parents to play, and 58% are talking with parents by using voices and words and waiting their turn to speak (Table 7).

My child enjoys being taken care of. For instance, Y/N N %
He/she smiles when I speak with him/her Yes 96 96.0
No 4 4.0
He/she speaks whenever I speak with him/her Yes 89 89.0
No 11 11.0
He/she speaks with me by using voices and words and waiting his/her turn Yes 58 58.0
No 42 42.0
He/she invites me to play with him/her Yes 89 89.0
No 11 11.0

Table 7: Reaction of child to being cared with him/her.

97% of the children follow the instructions of their parents when gesture-matching words are used, 95% when gestures are used, 92% when words and simple phrases are used, 90% when single stage instructions are given and 79% when two stage instructions are given (Table 8).

My child follows my instructions when I act as below. For instance, Y/N N %
When I use gestures Yes 95 95.0
No 5 5.0
When I use words that match with gestures Yes 97 97.0
No 3 3.0
When I use words and simple prepositional phrases Yes 92 92.0
No 8 8.0
When I give single commands Yes 90 90.0
No 10 10.0
When I give double commands Yes 79 79.0
No 21 21.0

Table 8: Children following directions.

Also, 97% of the children use different voices to express themselves: 93% use signs, 85% gestures and physical actions, and 66% use certain words for certain things (Table 9).

My child tells me what he/she feels and wants. For instance, Y/N N %
He/she uses different voices to express himself/herself Yes 97 97.0
No 3 3.0
Points out the things he/she wants Yes 93 93.0
No 7 7.0
He/she makes gestures Yes 85 85.0
No 15 15.0
He/she conducts physical actions Yes 85 85.0
No 15 15.0
Makes certain voices for certain things Yes 80 80.0
No 20 20.0
Uses certain words for certain things Yes 66 66.0
No 34 34.0
Speaks with prepositional phrases Yes 38 38.0
No 62 62.0
Speaks by sentences Yes 32 32.0
No 68 68.0

Table 9: Child's expression of feelings and wishes.

85% of the children say the names of their family members, 64% say the names of animals, 61% say the names of toys, 60% say the names of food, 54% say the names of daily routines and parts of the body, 43% say the names of clothes (Table 10).

My child can speak out words. For instance, Y/N N %
Names of family members Yes 85 85.0
No 15 15.0
Animals Yes 64 64.0
No 36 36.0
Food Yes 60 60.0
No 40 40.0
Toys Yes 61 61.0
No 39 39.0
Daily routines Yes 54 54.0
No 46 46.0
Body parts Yes 54 54.0
No 46 46.0
Clothes Yes 43 43.0
No 57 57.0

Table 10: Words child can say.

82% of the parents stated that the child's speech was understood by those living in the same household, 51% by other family members, 32% by those responsible for the care of the child, 20% by those who knew the child and 11% by those who did not know the child at all (Table 11).

The persons below can understand the speech of my child… G/S N %
Family members living in same household Generally 82 82.0
Sometimes 18 18.0
Other family members Generally 51 51.0
Sometimes 49 49.0
People who take care of child Generally 32 32.0
Sometimes 68 68.0
Acquaintances Generally 20 20.0
Sometimes 80 80.0
People who do not know him/her Generally 11 11.0
Sometimes 89 89.0

Table 11: People who understand the speech of child.

In addition, /b/ (84%), /d/ (84%) and /m/ (83%) phonemes were most correctly pronounced by the children (Table 12).

My child pronounces these voices correctly (Turkish Phonemes) Y/N N %
/b/ Yes 84 84.0
No 16 16.0
/c/ Yes 63 63.0
No 37 37.0
/ç/ Yes 61 61.0
No 39 39.0
/d/ Yes 84 84.0
No 16 16.0
/f/ Yes 60 60.0
No 40 40.0
/g/ Yes 62 62.0
No 38 38.0
/h/ Yes 60 60.0
No 40 40.0
/j/ Yes 44 44.0
No 56 56.0
/k/ Yes 51 51.0
No 49 49.0
/l/ Yes 55 55.0
No 45 45.0
/m/ Yes 83 83.0
No 17 17.0
/n/ Yes 79 79.0
No 21 21.0
/p/ Yes 63 63.0
No 37 37.0
/r/ Yes 24 24.0
No 76 76.0
/s/ Yes 43 43.0
No 57 57.0
/ş/ Yes 45 45.0
No 55 55.0
/t/ Yes 63 63.0
No 37 37.0
/v/ Yes 58 58.0
No 42 42.0
/y/ Yes 69 69.0
No 31 31.0
/z/ Yes 40 40.0
No 60 60.0

Table 12: Voices pronounced correctly by child.

The relationship between the pronunciations of the phonemes and the variables

Children’s gender and /t/ voice pronunciation was related (p<0.05).  /t/ voice can be pronounced by 63 children, of whom 74% are boys and 25.4% are girls. Among the 37 children who cannot pronounce it, 19% are boys and 18% are girls. There was no relationship between the gender of the children and the pronunciation of the other voices mentioned in the table (p>0.05). The ages of children and their ability/inability to pronounce /ç/, /d/ and /n/ were related (p<0.05). 16% of children are able to pronounce /ç/ in the range of 3.0-4.6 yrs and 45% in the range of 4.6-6.0 yrs. 23% of children are able to pronounce /d/ in the range of 3.0-4.6 yrs, 61% in the age of 4.6-6.0 yrs. 22% of children are able to pronounce /n/ in the range of 3.0-4.6 yrs, 57% of them in the age of 4.6-6.0 yrs. The ages of children and their ability/inability to pronounce other voices had no relations (p>0.05). The relationship between children's /b/ voice pronunciation and the time spent by the child at home with the siblings was found to be moderate at 0.401.

The relationship between the time spent by the children at home with the mother and the ability to pronounce the voice /c/ is 0.463, the ability to pronounce the voice /ç/ is 0.416, the ability to pronounce the voice /d/ is 0.462, the ability to pronounce the voice /j/ is 0.402, the ability to pronounce the voice /r/ is 0.510 and the ability to pronounce the voice /s/ is 0.449 – all are moderate. The relationship between the time the child spends watching TV at the weekends and the ability to pronounce the voice /f/ is 0.391, and the ability to pronounce the voice /ş/ is 0.412 and the ability to pronounce the voice /m/ is 0.404 – all are moderate.

The relationship between the time spent by the child at home with the siblings and the ability to pronounce the voice /h/ is 0.373, the ability to pronounce the voice /k/ is 0.423, the ability to pronounce the voice /l/ is 0.518, the ability to pronounce the voice /n/ is 0.406, and the ability to pronounce the voice /p/ is 0.481 – all are moderate. The relationship between the time spent by the children at home with the father and the ability to pronounce the voice /t/ is 0.419 and the ability to pronounce the voice /v/ is 0.424 – both are moderate.

Conclusion

Language development is a complex process that begins with the birth. Although there are basic elements that are determined in the language development process, they may vary according to the child. Factors such as psychosocial development, biological development, social communication development, neurological development, and intelligence are influential in the development process. The language itself is examined in two parts. Receptive language is the perception and interpretation of what is said by the listener. On the other hand, expressive language is produced by the speaker to convey his/her feelings and thoughts. In the process of language development, these two factors need to develop in harmony with age. Gender variable plays an important role in language development. Girls' vocabulary is larger than boys [9,10].

In our study, 66% of the children who participated in the survey were boys and 34% were girls. According to this data, gender can be considered a risk factor for delayed speech.

Age variation in language and speech development influences other factors in the child's development as well as language development. In a study, it was stated that chronological age and language development were directly proportional [11]. Considering the age range of children participating in our study, 35% are between the ages of 3 and 4 yrs 6 months and 65% are between the ages of 4 yrs 6 months and 6 yrs. The majority of the children participating in the study are over the age of 4 yrs.

Pre-school period is a period when child development is at a rapid level. Language development is very important before literacy activities are started. The ability of the child to express himself /herself properly, to speak properly, to correct speech disorders is supported by activities in pre-school education [12]. 76% of the children who participated in the school had pre-school education, whereas 24% did not have. The number of children who received pre-school education is higher than the number who did not. Pre-school language training activities in speech therapy help to correct speech impairment.

The language development and acquisition process that start in the family environment continues with the factors such as television, computer, technological devices, friends, environment, school and social life relations [13]. The multiplicity of foreign cartoons and their translations have a negative effect on the conversation. 74% of the children who participated in this study said that their favorite program on TV was cartoons; 17% said children's programs and 9% said movies. According to this data, we can say that the cartoons have a role in speech delay. According to a study [14], if the number of the individuals in a family is low, the interest that the parents will show to their children and the time they will spare for their children will be much higher. The number of siblings is also effective on language development. According to the advocated view, the care that the child receives and the quality of time have more impact than the number of siblings. The level of parental education affects the quality of the time spent with the child. Parents with a high educational level are able to create positively influential environments for children’s language acquisition and expressive language development [6,11,15] by finding positive behaviors in the development of the child. The basic stages of language are similar, but the social environment and social interaction are important factors in the speed of language acquisition. Healthy communication between the mother and the child affects the language development process positively, especially with the parents in the family [16]. It is also shown that the quality time spent with the father is a factor in language development [17].

Some researchers [18] investigated the effects of parental education on children's language development and reported that maternal love affects the speed of voice production. It is also evident from the study of our work that when parents take care of children with delayed speech; 96% of them smile, 89% speak with their parents when they speak, 89% invite parents to play games and 58% speak with their parents using voices and words and waiting for their turn to speak.

When we look at the data in our study, we can say that the duration of television watching is one of the important reasons for speech delay. Studies on the subject indicate that the length of time spent watching television and using computer leads to a lack of verbal communication [19].

The characteristics of children's family structures lead to changes in the level of children's learning from TV and their level of influence. According to some studies, children in developed countries watch TV at least 3 h and 38 mins every day [20]. This result is consistent with our research result. Children spend 2.79 h at weekends, 2.34 h on weekdays watching TV and 0.94 h on computers or tablets. A computer or tablet, which is common in almost every household, appears to reduce the time children spend on TV; however, the time children spend on a screen does not seem to change at all. General impacts of television on children are learning about gender roles, relationships with the opposite sex, parent-child relationships, violence tendency, reading, thinking, success, creativity, self-perception and language development. As it can be seen, these impacts can be positive or negative. Among the impacts that negatively affect language development in children who watch television are the lack or delay of reading habits, an increase in the tendency for violence, difficulty in social relations, and distractibility [21]. In our study, it was also found that there was a moderate correlation between the times spent watching TV during weekends and pronunciation of some phonemes (/f/, /ş/, /m/).  The increasing use of erroneous and long-lasting technology (computer, tablet, etc.) in recent years constructs the basis for speech delay in early childhood. Especially important is the effective use of speech between the ages 0 and 3, which is the most critical period for language and speech development, by increasing educational verbal communication in the home environment and by keeping children away from intense visual stimuli as much as possible.

References

  1. Feldman HM (2005) Evaluation and management of language and speech disorders in preschool children. Pediatr Rev 26: 131-142.
  2. Weismer G (2006) Speech Disorders (Chapter 4), Handbook of Psycholinguistics (Second Edition) 93-124.
  3. Feldman HM, Messick C (2009) Language and Speech Disorders (Chapter 72) Developmental-Behavioral Pediatrics (Fourth Edition) 717-729.
  4. Tager-Flusberg H, Seery AM (2013) Early Development of Speech and Language: Cognitive, Behavioral, and Neural Systems, Neural Circuit Development and Function in the Brain 315-330.
  5. Feldman HM, Messick C (2008) Language and Speech Disorders Chapter 13 Developmental-Behavioral Pediatrics 467-482.
  6. Baxter J (2010) An Exploration of the Timing and Nature of Parental Time with 4–5-year-olds Using Australian Children’s Time Use Data. Research Paper No 45. Melbourne: AIFS 10: 1-18.
  7. Zimmerman IL, Steiner VG, Pond RE (2011) Preschool Language Scale–Fifth Edition (PLS–5) San Antonio TX: Pearson.
  8. Sahli AS, Belgin E (2017) Adaptation, validity  and reliability of the Preschool Language Scale–Fifth Edition (PLS–5) in the Turkish context: The Turkish Preschool Language Scale–5 (TPLS–5) Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 98: 143–9.
  9. Brady N, Marquis J, Fleming K, McLean L (2004) Prelinguistic predictor of language growth in children with developmental disabilities. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 47:663-77.
  10. Campbell T, Dollaghan C, Rockette H, Paradise J, Feldman H, et al. (2003) Risk factors for speech delay of unknown origin in 3-year-old children. Child Dev 74: 346-57.
  11. Dale PS, Price TS, Bishop DV, Plomin R (2003) Outcomes of early language delay: I. Predicting persistent and transient language difficulties at 3 and 4 years. J Speech Lang Hear Res 46:544-60.
  12. Law J, Garrett Z, Nye C (2004) The efficacy of treatment for children with developmental speech and language delay/disorder: A meta-analysis. J Speech Lang Hear Res 47:924-43.
  13. Yaglı A (2013) The role of cartoons in the education and social development of children: Caillou and Pepee example. Turkish studies 8: 707-19.
  14.  Erdogan S, Bekir SH, Aras ES (2005) An investigation of the effect of some factors on the level of language development of children in the 5-6 Age group undergoing the preschool in lower socioeconomic regions. CU Journal of Social Sciences 14: 231-46.
  15. Rescorla L, Schwartz E (1990) Outcome of toddlers with specific expressive language delay. Appl Psycholinguist 11: 393-407.
  16. Karacan E (2000) Language Development in Infants and Children. J Clin Psychiatry 4: 263-8.
  17. Turkoglu B, Celikoz N, Uslu M (2013) Opinions of children who are at 3-6 years of age about their qualified time perceptions. J Educ Train Stud 2: 54-71.
  18. Feldman MA, Case L, Rincover A, Towns F, Betel J, et al. (1989) Parent Education Project III. Increasing affection and responsivity in developmentally handicapped mothers: Component analysis, generalization, and effects on child language. J Appl Behav Anal 22: 211–22.
  19. Vandewater EA, Rideout VJ, Wartella EA, Huang X, Lee JH, et al. (2007) Digital childhood: electronic media and technology use among infants, toddlers, and preschoolers. Pediatrics 119: 1006-15.
  20. Erturk YD (2004) Family in the interaction of children and television. I. International Conference on Children and Communication 271-7.
  21. Buyukbaykal G (2007) Effects of television on children. Istanbul University Communication Faculty Magazine  31-44.

Citation: Keten S, Sahli AS, Kaya M (2018) Evaluation of Home Communication Skills in Children with Speech Delay . J Speech Pathol Ther 3: 137. DOI: 10.4172/2472-5005.1000137

Copyright: © 2018 Keten S, et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Top